Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Perceived difficulty and appropriateness of decision making by General Practitioners: a systematic review of scenario studies

Abstract

Background

Health-care quality in primary care depends largely on the appropriateness of General Practitioners’ (GPs; Primary Care or Family Physicians) decisions, which may be influenced by how difficult they perceive decisions to be. Patient scenarios (clinical or case vignettes) are widely used to investigate GPs’ decision making. This review aimed to identify the extent to which perceived decision difficulty, decision appropriateness, and their relationship have been assessed in scenario studies of GPs’ decision making; identify possible determinants of difficulty and appropriateness; and investigate the relationship between difficulty and appropriateness.

Methods

MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsycINFO, the Cochrane Library and Web of Science were searched for scenario studies of GPs’ decision making. One author completed article screening. Ten percent of titles and abstracts were checked by an independent volunteer, resulting in 91% agreement. Data on decision difficulty and appropriateness were extracted by one author and descriptively synthesised. Chi-squared tests were used to explore associations between decision appropriateness, decision type and decision appropriateness assessment method.

Results

Of 152 included studies, 66 assessed decision appropriateness and five assessed perceived difficulty. While no studies assessed the relationship between perceived difficulty and appropriateness, one study objectively varied the difficulty of the scenarios and assessed the relationship between a measure of objective difficulty and appropriateness. Across 38 studies where calculations were possible, 62% of the decisions were appropriate as defined by the appropriateness standard used. Chi-squared tests identified statistically significant associations between decision appropriateness, decision type and decision appropriateness assessment method. Findings suggested a negative relationship between decision difficulty and appropriateness, while interventions may have the potential to reduce perceived difficulty.

Conclusions

Scenario-based research into GPs’ decisions rarely considers the relationship between perceived decision difficulty and decision appropriateness. The links between these decisional components require further investigation.

Peer Review reports

Background

There is extensive evidence demonstrating that patients do not always receive the highest quality care possible [1]. In primary care, General Practitioners (GPs, also known as Primary Care or Family Physicians) are largely responsible for making clinical decisions concerning their patients, so their decisions have a significant impact on health care quality. Decision appropriateness in this context can be defined as the extent to which clinical decisions made by GPs are in accordance with a standard such as an evidence-based clinical guideline (although it is important to note that appropriateness can be conceptualised in many different ways, and guidelines are one of a number of standards that can be used to assess appropriateness). Research into the appropriateness of GPs’ decisions often involves the use of patient scenarios (clinical or case vignettes), where GPs review patient descriptions and simulate the decisions they would make in a real consultation. Decision appropriateness is then assessed by comparing the decisions to an appropriate decision defined by a standard such as a clinical guideline.

Scenario methods can also be used to identify factors influencing the appropriateness of GPs’ decisions. Cognitive psychology theory suggests that a key determinant of a decision outcome is the difficulty of that decision [2],[3]: perceived difficulty with decision making is therefore likely to be related to the appropriateness of GPs decisions. There is currently no widely accepted definition of perceived difficulty, but it has been described as being experienced when a decision maker finds it difficult to choose a certain course of action, or when it is unclear which course of action best meets a decision makers’ goals [2]. Perceived difficulty can be assessed by asking decision makers to use a scale to rate the difficulty experienced when making a decision [2].

Although all individuals will have at some point experienced difficulty when making a decision, there has been little scientific study of this concept [2],[3]: as such, there is currently no theoretical consensus on the characteristics of a decision that make it difficult [3]. However, there is some indication that the complexity of the decision may be important: increasing complexity may cause difficulty whereby complex decisions involving consideration of many factors are perceived as difficult [3],[4]. In focus group discussions based on patient scenarios, GPs described their difficulty with deciding whether to refer older patients for colon cancer screening [5]. One of the main sources of difficulty cited was the number of factors which had to be taken into account [5].

Although it might be intuitively appealing to expect that increased decision difficulty leads to less appropriate decisions, it might also be expected that making difficult decisions involves the use of a more in-depth analytic decision making process and leads to more appropriate decisions. Although one previous systematic review of 30 scenario studies considered the appropriateness of GPs’ decisions [6], to our knowledge there have been no previous reviews of the perceived difficulty of decisions made by GPs, the factors that might influence this, or the relationship between the difficulty and appropriateness of GPs’ decisions. Clearly, many decisions made by GPs are difficult and it may not always possible to make them easier. However, identifying health conditions, decision types, or patient characteristics that are associated with increased difficulty and quantifying the relationship between difficulty and appropriateness could be important for enhancing the knowledge base relating to GPs’ decision making processes, and informing strategies aimed at improving the appropriateness of clinical decisions, and thus patient care.

Consequently, the present review aims to: a) identify the extent to which perceived decision difficulty, decision appropriateness, and the relationship between the two have been assessed in scenario studies of GPs’ decision making; b) identify possible determinants of difficulty and appropriateness within the primary care setting; and c) investigate the relationship between difficulty and appropriateness in the context of GPs’ decision making.

Methods

This is a systematic review of published studies. This study did not recruit any participants, but involved secondary analysis of papers that are in the public domain: review by an ethics committee was therefore not applicable to this study. This article reflects the relevant components of the PRISMA checklist for the reporting of systematic reviews [7].

Inclusion criteria

Studies eligible for inclusion used patient scenarios. To ensure study selection was systematic, a definition of patient scenarios was created, similar to that of Veloski and colleagues [8]: a patient scenario is “a brief description of a patient designed to represent an actual primary care consultation”. Studies which stated that scenarios were used but which did not fit this definition were excluded. There were no restrictions on the scenario format or delivery method. Studies using just one scenario were excluded as they could not have compared responses to different scenarios to identify determinants of decision difficulty or appropriateness. Qualitative and think aloud studies were excluded as they do not involve quantitative assessment of decision difficulty or appropriateness in situations which reflect actual consultations. Interview studies which were quantitative in nature (i.e. where an interviewer was present but GPs were given or shown scenarios and asked to give quantitative responses) were included. Studies investigating end of life decision making were excluded because of the many ethical, legal and societal issues inherent in these decisions which are unlikely to be present in the everyday clinical decisions made by GPs working in the community [9].

Participants must have included fully qualified GPs working in community settings. Eligible studies required participants to make a clinical decision (a decision made with respect to patient care [10]) regarding the patients presented in the scenarios. Studies involving non-definitive decisions (for example, where participants rated their willingness to prescribe) were included. Studies which collected any other types of outcome measures (such as estimates of treatment effectiveness) or which did not elicit decisions specifically relating to the patients in the scenarios (for example, where participants rated treatment appropriateness) were excluded, on the basis that participants were not making clinical decisions for patients as they would in actual practice. There were no restrictions on response format.

Search methods

Electronic searches were carried out in MEDLINE (1946 to week 1 of February 2012) and Embase (1980 to week 6 of 2012) using the OVID interface, and in PsycINFO, the Cochrane Library and Web of Science on February 14th 2012. A search strategy (included in Additional file 1) was designed in MEDLINE and modified accordingly for use in the additional databases. No language restrictions were imposed. GPs are labelled in various ways in countries other than the UK, while many different terms can be used to refer to the use of patient scenarios: this was reflected in our search strategies. The journal Medical Decision Making was hand-searched for relevant conference abstracts from 2009-April 2012. The reference lists of included studies were reviewed.

Data collection

One author (NM) screened titles and abstracts retrieved by electronic searching; 10% were screened by an independent volunteer (Brian Power), resulting in 91% agreement. As an additional check, all authors independently screened a set of the same 10 titles and abstracts. Screening disagreements were resolved by discussion. One author (NM) screened full-text articles using a form (included in Additional file 2) designed using Cochrane guidance [11] and which was piloted by all authors. Study eligibility doubts were resolved by discussion with all authors. One author (NM) extracted data using a form (included in Additional file 3) developed using Cochrane guidance [11] and relevant literature [6],[10],[12],[13] and which was piloted by all authors. Data pertaining to study characteristics, participants, patient scenario construction, outcome measures, and results were extracted and stored electronically.

Data analysis

Where relevant details were not available in the paper, efforts were made to obtain them by contacting study authors. The number of studies which assessed decision difficulty, decision appropriateness, or the possible relationship between the two was counted. For a study to be categorised as having assessed decision appropriateness, the appropriateness of the decisions made must have been explicitly assessed in reference to some standard, or scenarios must have been designed according to some standard such that the appropriate decision was evident. This review aimed to be inclusive and gather studies which had used a range of approaches to assess decision appropriateness: therefore, we did not restrict appropriateness assessment (for example to national standards), but rather accepted all standards. Studies which discussed guidelines or another standard in relation to their results, but which did not explicitly assess decision appropriateness and provide results for this, were categorised as not having assessed decision appropriateness.

Heterogeneity between studies prevented correlational analysis of factors associated with decision difficulty, so studies were descriptively analysed. However, it was possible to pool the data from the studies which assessed decision appropriateness, by focussing on the number of decisions deemed appropriate. The number of appropriate decisions (defined by the standard used) was calculated by NM where this was possible, as follows:

Where the overall number of appropriate decisions across all scenarios was reported, this was taken directly from the paper.

Where the number of appropriate decisions per scenario was reported, these details were taken directly from the paper and summed to create an overall total.

Where percentages of appropriate decisions were reported, these details were taken directly from the paper and used to calculate the number of appropriate decisions, either overall or per scenario and then summed to create an overall total.

The specific types of decisions made and the decision appropriateness assessment methods used were categorised for each study by one author (NM), and any doubts were resolved by discussion with all authors. To categorise decision type, NM extracted decision details from all studies, grouped them under headings such as prescribing, and headings were then agreed by all authors. All authors agreed on the final categories listed in Table 1. The diagnosis category included diagnostic decisions; the screening or testing category included decisions involving screening, examination or diagnostic test ordering; the treatment or management category included decisions about treatment and further patient management. Since the latter two categories covered a wide range of decisions, these were sub-categorised as indicated in Table 1. The categories of decision appropriateness assessment method in Table 1 reflect the methods used to determine appropriate and inappropriate decisions.

Table 1 Associations between decision appropriateness and decision type and appropriateness assessment standard

Chi-squared tests were used to explore associations between decision appropriateness and decision type and method used to determine decision appropriateness. The decisions within the studies were already categorised for these analyses, based on the categorisation of the overall studies from which the decisions came. Categorising decisions for the analysis was therefore based on the already agreed categories for the studies and so validity checking of the decision categorisations was not thought to be necessary. Data were analysed using SPSS version 20.

Results

Study selection

The search retrieved 4657 articles, and 185 articles (reporting 152 studies) were included in the review. Full details of the study selection process are provided in Figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flow chart of identification and selection of included studies. Note: GP = General Practitioner; HCP = Health Care Professional.

Characteristics of included studies

The majority of included studies (119) used questionnaires. Twelve of these had additional components: 10 were nested within larger studies (five within randomised controlled trials (RCTs), two within observational studies, one within a pre-post intervention study, one within a before-and-after study, and one within a standardised patient study), and two were factorial experiments. Of the remaining 33 studies, 25 used interviews. Nine of these had additional components: one was nested within an RCT, and eight were factorial experiments. Of the remaining eight studies, two used questionnaire and/or interview surveys, two were crossover experiments which used a balanced block design, one was a simulated cluster RCT, and three were simulated decision making experiments.

Studies were published between 1974 and 2011, with 59% published during or after the year 2000. Thirty seven percent were conducted in the USA, 20% in the UK, 9% in Canada, 5% in Australia and 5% in the Netherlands. The remainder were carried out in different or multiple countries. The studies focussed on a wide range of clinical behaviours, such as diagnosis, test-ordering, prescribing, providing advice, and referral. Further study descriptives are included in Table 2. The majority of studies (74%) used written scenarios which were delivered via paper questionnaires. The rest used scenarios presented on computers or via video. Where specified, various different sources were used to generate scenario content, including clinical experience, real patient data, the literature, and clinical guidelines. A summary of the key features of each study is included in Additional file 4.

Table 2 Key characteristics of 152 included studies

Decision appropriateness

Sixty-six of the 152 studies assessed decision appropriateness, 43 using guidelines. The number of appropriate decisions could be extracted or calculated for 38 studies: overall, 58666 clinical decisions were made, and 62% were appropriate (mean 58%, SD 24%, range 6% to 100%). Pearson Chi-squared tests found statistically significant associations between decision appropriateness and decision type and decision appropriateness assessment method (Table 1). Across the three analyses, decision appropriateness was

Highest for screening or testing decisions and lowest for treatment or management decisions

Highest for test-ordering decisions and lowest for treatment other than prescribing decisions

Highest when literature was used to assess decision appropriateness, and lowest when actual diagnosis was used

Some studies contributed many decisions (for example, one study [14],[15] contributed 20631 decisions), and some categories represented one study. Sensitivity analyses were carried out whereby the analyses were repeated after removing decisions from studies contributing 1000 decisions or more to a category from that category, and categories representing one study. These analyses were not pre-specified. Significant associations between decision appropriateness and study characteristics remained, indicating that the findings are robust.

Table 3 summarises the decision types investigated, and decision appropriateness assessment and analysis methods used in the remaining 28 studies where the number of appropriate decision could not be calculated. This shows that 79% of these studies focussed on treatment or management decisions, with 64% specifically focussed on prescribing. Additionally, 50% of the studies used guidelines only to assess decision appropriateness. The studies used a range of methods to analyse decision appropriateness, such as calculating agreement with the decision appropriateness assessment standard (for example, percentage agreement with an expert panel’s decisions), or calculating quality scores (for example, scoring GPs a point for an appropriate decision then calculating a mean score) (Table 3). For 43% of the studies, proportions of decisions were calculated but the way in which the data were presented prevented inclusion in the Chi-squared analyses.

Table 3 Decision types assessed, decision appropriateness assessment standards used, and decision appropriate analysis methods of 28 studies not included in the Chi-squared analyses

Decision difficulty and the relationship between decision difficulty and decision appropriateness

Five of the 152 studies assessed the perceived difficulty of the GPs’ decisions [16]-[20]. One did not report the difficulty data [18], leaving four studies to be analysed. The key features of these studies are summarised in Table 4: further details can be found in the summary of key study features in Additional file 4. As Table 4 indicates, all studies differed greatly in terms of the study type, the health condition investigated, and how the outcome of decision difficulty was measured and the outcome data summarised.

Table 4 Key findings from four studies assessing perceived decision difficulty

Efforts were made to contact authors of all four studies to obtain scenario content, and the scenarios used by Lynggaard and Strandgaard and by Short and colleagues were obtained. The number of pieces of information in these scenarios were counted by one author (NM) and used as an indicator of complexity. The scenarios used by Lynggaard and Strandgaard, the number of pieces of information, the percentage of GPs who prescribed and who perceived the decision as easy are include in Table 5. As Table 5 shows, the two scenarios containing eight pieces of information were perceived as easy by a greater proportion of GPs than the three scenarios containing nine pieces. Therefore, clinical situations containing more information were more often perceived as difficult. However, it is not clear whether this is due to either the number of pieces of information per se, what these pieces of information were, how they have been combined in the scenarios, another reason we have not considered, or whether this is simply a chance finding. This could not be explored further using Short and colleagues’ scenarios since difficulty was not assessed per scenario.

Table 5 Scenario details and percentage of GPs prescribing and who perceived the prescribing decision as easy for the scenarios used by Lynggaard and Strandgaard [19]

One study, by Kostopoulou and colleagues, explicitly investigated the relationship between decision difficulty and decision appropriateness [22]. However, perceived decision difficulty was not assessed: various sources were used to derive 10 factors which may cause diagnostic difficulty, and the presence of these was varied across scenarios [22]. This study therefore focussed on objective difficulty, which can be defined as an independent assessment of the difficulty of a decision task. Participants (63 GPs and 21 residents) made diagnostic and management decisions, and decision appropriateness was assessed using expert panel diagnoses and guideline recommendations [22]. The correlation between decision difficulty and appropriateness for the diagnostic decisions was reported. There was a significant negative correlation: as the number of difficulty factors increased, the number of appropriate diagnostic decisions decreased [22].

The four previous studies which assessed perceived decision difficulty provide some support for there being a negative relationship between perceived difficulty and appropriateness, although the relationship was not explicitly assessed:

Bonetti and colleagues conducted an RCT investigating the effectiveness of audit and feedback and educational reminder messages in improving lumbar spine x-ray ordering decisions for back pain [16]. There was no effect of the educational reminders on difficulty. However, the GPs who received audit and feedback found the post-intervention decisions significantly less difficult than those who did not [16] (Table 4). There was a significant correlation between difficulty and post-intervention decisions [16]: as the difficulty score increased, the number of decisions to order an x-ray (which were mostly inappropriate) increased. However, difficulty did not enter a regression model predicting decisions [16] (instead, decisions were predicted by attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioural control, from the Theory of Planned Behaviour [23]).

Carroll and colleagues conducted an RCT investigating the effectiveness of a knowledge translation intervention in improving referral decisions for women with different cancer risks [17]. The authors found that a significantly greater number of appropriate decisions were made in the intervention group than the control group at post-intervention and, as indicated previously, that difficulty was lower [17] (Table 4).

Short and colleagues conducted a before-and-after study investigating the effectiveness of a computerised decision support tool in improving aspirin prescribing decisions for stroke patients with complicating co-morbidity [20]. After the intervention the authors found stronger agreement that the decisions were easy and an overall shift towards prescribing (which was mostly appropriate) [20] (Table 4).

Lynggaard and Strandgaard conducted a questionnaire survey investigating decisions to start treatment for mild to moderate hypertension and commented that according to the New Zealand Core Services Committee Guidelines, it was appropriate to prescribe for scenarios 1–3, and not to prescribe for scenarios 4 and 5 in their study [19]. The percentage of appropriate decisions per scenario was 96%, 85%, 96%, 44%, and 37% respectively (Tables 4 and 5). Comparing this to the percentages of decisions considered easy (83%, 67%, 80%, 50% and 50% respectively) shows that when fewer GPs made an appropriate decision, more GPs considered that decision difficult.

Discussion

Of 152 scenario studies which investigated GPs’ clinical decisions, 66 assessed decision appropriateness, 5 assessed perceived decision difficulty, and one assessed the relationship between objective difficulty and appropriateness. Therefore, the appropriateness of GPs’ decision making has been assessed to a much greater extent than difficulty, while the potentially important relationship between difficulty and appropriateness has rarely been studied in this context. This agrees with the wider psychological literature on decision making which, as stated in the introduction, has rarely investigated decision difficulty and its relationship with decision outcomes [2],[3].

Overall, 62% of the decisions made across studies were appropriate, which is consistent with the hypothesis that there are deficiencies in clinical decision making. However, the range of appropriate decisions across included studies was considerably large (6% to 100%): it is therefore unclear to what extent this overall figure relates to real practice. The wide range in appropriateness found may reflect a multitude of factors: for example, the different patient groups and decision types studied in the included papers, the varying levels of detail provided in the scenarios, or the different methods used to assess decision appropriateness. It is often argued that scenarios have limited ecological validity (the extent to which an aspect of the real world is represented, in this case the real world of clinical practice) [24]. For example, scenarios are often missing certain components of real consultations, such as information gathered from non-verbal cues, although video scenarios can help alleviate some of these problems. We cannot comment on the ecological validity of the included scenarios, as assessing this was beyond scope of this review. However, many studies show that there is wide variation in the quality of primary care delivered in practices across the UK [25]-[28], and our results agree with this.

Most decisions concerned treatment or management, most of which concerned prescribing. This is also true for the 28 studies where the number of appropriate decisions could not be calculated. Many actual consultations involve prescribing: for example, each GP in Scotland issues an average of 70 prescriptions a day [29]. Prescribing is therefore one of the most common decisions made by GPs, and so it is not surprising that this decision type would be frequently studied using scenarios. Only 60% of the prescribing decisions in the Chi-squared analyses were appropriate, and further evidence suggests that inappropriate prescribing does occur in primary care [30]-[34]. This indicates that decision appropriateness is suboptimal for the decisions most frequently made by GPs: further work is required to improve these decisions.

Only 47% of the advice-giving decisions were appropriate, which is again suboptimal. The literature on prescribing for upper respiratory tract infections (one of the most common primary care consultation types [32],[35]) suggests that GPs prescribe rather than provide appropriate advice because they assume this will save time [36],[37]. This suggests that GPs’ decisions are not solely influenced by the relevant evidence base: practical constraints, such as physician time, effort, and reward, are also important. Indeed, this agrees with previous quantitative and qualitative research indicating that ‘non-clinical’ or contextual factors, such as the time available, workload, years of experience, and patient demand (or GPs’ perceptions of demand) do influence the decisions that GPs make [38]-[46].

Both ours and a previous review [6] found that decision appropriateness is typically assessed by comparing decisions with guidelines. This seems logical - guidelines were created to improve and standardise practice [47], so could be considered a highly appropriate standard for evaluating practice. In addition, our review found that conclusions regarding decision appropriateness differ depending on how appropriateness is assessed: in comparison to guidelines, studies using other methods potentially overestimate decision appropriateness. However, guidelines may not always reflect appropriate decisions in the real world. As most GPs are well aware, guideline recommendations usually apply to the ‘average patient’ and it is the responsibility of the clinician to use the recommendation in conjunction with their knowledge of the patient to form a clinical opinion, and then discuss options with the patient. However, GPs cannot use this strategy in scenario studies: when assessing decision appropriateness using guidelines, the decision made is compared to the guideline recommendation and rated as appropriate or inappropriate. The results of this review further emphasise a point made in the introduction, namely that there are numerous ways in which health care quality can be conceptualised: it is possible that these other methods allow for broader definitions of appropriate decisions. In the studies using expert panels, the panels were convened to consider the specific scenarios used, and therefore made recommendations based on these specific clinical situations. This may have contributed to the difference in percentage of appropriate decisions. In future studies, it may be advantageous to use a combination of methods.

Related to this, the fact that we could not include 28 of the studies that assessed decision appropriateness in the Chi-squared analyses indicates not only that there are different ways of conceptualising decision appropriateness, but also that there are different ways of analysing and reporting the results of studies that investigate this. Aside from the 12 studies where data could not be included in our analyses due to the format in which the data were presented, the most common analysis methods in these studies involved calculating agreement with an appropriateness standard such as an expert panel, or calculating quality scores. This highlights the difficulty with synthesising scenario data: there are no widely accepted reporting standards, therefore a variety of methods are used.

As mentioned in the introduction, there has been little research into difficulty and its potential determinants in the psychological literature: the present review shows that this is also the case in clinical decision-making scenario literature. This is an important gap in the research: there could be many factors which contribute to increased difficulty with real clinical decisions. These could be patient-related factors such as the nature of the complaint, the presence of contradictory symptoms or signs, the severity of problem, or whether the patient requests a particular treatment; GP or practice-related factors such as the number of years qualified, the practice case mix, or typical workloads; or other factors such as the number of possible treatment or management options that can be selected and their potential outcomes, or the time of day or day of the week. Further studies are needed which investigate or manipulate these factors and compare difficulty across situations to identify factors that may influence difficulty.

The few studies identified which had assessed perceived decision difficulty did so for a variety of different consultation types and health problems, and measured and summarised decision difficulty in a variety of ways. Despite this variability, the three intervention studies indicated that interventions aimed at improving GPs’ decisions have the potential to reduce perceived difficulty [16],[17],[20]. Only one study provided difficulty scores per scenario [19]: in the other studies, perceived difficulty was assessed for the set of scenarios as a whole [20], or difficulty scores were summed across scenarios [16],[17] (see Table 4). This is a key finding: studies where difficulty scores are assessed per scenario afford the opportunity to investigate which specific scenario factors influence the perceived difficulty scores. As our review has found, these types of studies in particular are lacking.

Although no studies explicitly assessed the relationship between perceived difficulty and decision appropriateness, one study identified a significant negative association between objective difficulty and appropriateness [22]. In another study [19], the more information the scenarios contained, the greater the proportion of participants who perceived scenarios as difficult, supporting the view that as complexity increases, difficulty increases [3]-[5]. Here, complexity could be regarded as an objective measure of difficulty. Importantly, as more GPs perceived scenarios as difficult and complexity increased, fewer GPs made appropriate decisions. However, it is important to note that it is not clear that increasing the amount of information leads to increased perceived difficulty. This result could be due to the nature or relevance of the information in the scenarios or could be a chance finding. Further research is therefore required before any firm conclusions can be drawn. Although the relationship between perceived difficulty and appropriateness was not explicitly assessed in the four studies which assessed perceived difficulty, the results provide some support for there being a negative relationship: where decision difficulty was greater, it was generally the case that fewer appropriate decisions were made [16],[17],[19],[20].

Recommendations for future research

On the basis of these results, hypotheses can be generated regarding the relationships between objective decision difficulty, perceived decision difficulty, and decision appropriateness, in the context of GPs’ clinical decision making. Specifically, our findings are in accordance with the hypotheses that a) as objective difficulty increases, perceived difficulty increases; b) as objective difficulty increases, appropriateness decreases; and c) as perceived difficulty increases, appropriateness decreases.

Further robustly-designed studies are necessary to test these hypotheses, especially given that studies in this review have indicated that interventions may have the potential to reduce perceived decision difficulty. This further research should involve assessment of perceived difficulty, as well as objective measurement of the difficulty of the scenarios used, perhaps using panels of GPs. Objective difficulty could also be manipulated in the manner of Kostopoulou and colleagues. It is also important going forward for researchers to create scenarios in a systematic fashion such that specific aspects can be related to difficulty and appropriateness, perhaps using regression techniques: this would help identify determinants of difficulty and appropriateness. Finally, future studies could use both guidelines and expert panels to assess appropriateness in a comprehensive manner.

We suggested in the introduction that a decision perceived as difficult may instigate a more effortful decision making process than decisions perceived as easy, leading to a more appropriate decision, The results of this review are not in line with this hypothesis, However, the negative relationships suggested by the review results may be attenuated in scenario studies as opposed to studies of real decisions, as participants in scenario studies may see this as a good opportunity to instigate an effortful decision process. Investigation of real decisions is therefore also important, especially given the context of ever-growing complexity in primary care: GPs are expected to be aware of an increasing number of guideline recommendations, and to incorporate patient preferences into their decision strategies [48]. Multi-morbidity also increases complexity: this can result in competing issues, and can make following guideline recommendations, which are typically written for a single morbidity, increasingly complicated.

Strengths and limitations

The comprehensive nature of the search allowed studies from a range of countries to be included in this review, increasing the generalisability of our findings. Our study also included data from a wide range of GPs’ decisions, allowing results to be applicable to the range of common decisions faced in primary care. In addition, a large number of scenarios were included (data from over 58000 individual clinical decisions) increasing the power of our study to detect clinically important differences, should they exist. However, as previously indicated, issues with the ecological validity of scenarios mean that the extent to which our findings are generalizable to real practice in unclear. Nevertheless, our results agree with the multitude of studies showing that there is variation in quality of care.

There are also a number of limitations. Since participants in these studies responded to multiple scenarios, their decisions may not be independent: this was not adjusted for in the Chi-squared analyses. The significant associations found were highly statistically significant, which occurs when large sample sizes are used: this discussion has, therefore, focussed on the trends in the data. Although a Chi-squared analysis is limited, we considered it the optimal method of analysing our results as of all the analysis methods considered, it was the method that enabled data from the greatest proportion of the studies which assessed decision appropriateness to be analysed. These analyses did not include two further factors which may be associated with decision appropriateness: patient group, and whether appropriate decisions involved the GPs performing an action (e.g. ordering a necessary test) or not performing an action (e.g. not prescribing unnecessary antibiotics). These were excluded due to difficulties with study categorisation. As discussed, it was not possible to calculate numbers of appropriate decisions for 28 of the 66 studies which assessed decision appropriateness, so we were unable to aggregate all the data. This highlights an important issue with respect to outcome reporting in scenario research: many different methods are used, preventing data synthesis. It is important that researchers, reviewers and editors strive for consistency.

Most of the analyses were carried out by one reviewer: however, issues were resolved by discussion with the whole author team. Another issue arose when determining whether the same studies were being reported in certain articles. Articles were treated as reporting the same study when this was clear. However, we acknowledge that these difficulties may have resulted in double-counting of some studies. In all but two instances where there was a concern, only one of the papers had decision appropriateness data that were used in the Chi-squared analyses. These two instances involved the diagnostic decision data for depression from the studies by Freund and colleagues (121 appropriate of 128 decisions) and Frayne and colleagues (155 appropriate of 243 decisions), and the diagnostic, prescribing and test-ordering data for Coronary Heart Disease from the studies by Arber and colleagues (873 appropriate of total 1536 decisions) and Shackelton-Piccolo and colleagues (942 appropriate of total 1835 decisions). It is also possible that we may not have retrieved all relevant studies: however, we attempted to combat this by creating as comprehensive a search strategy as possible.

The final issue concerns the validity of patient scenarios as a proxy method of studying GPs’ decision making. Evidence from rigorous studies carried out by Peabody and colleagues suggests that scenarios are a valid proxy measure of clinical behaviour [49],[50], but this is not conclusive [10],[51]. This may be because there is no standardised method for developing valid scenarios, resulting in wide variation in how rigorously scenarios are validated before use. However, scenario studies are arguably less ethically challenging than methods requiring direct observation of consultations, and considerably less resource intensive. Therefore, it seems sensible for researchers to carry out further work to explore the relationship between difficulty and appropriateness firstly using valid scenarios designed to reflect real practice, such that the results of scenario studies are useful for informing practice, and then in real practice, where studies are considerably more resource-intensive and complex. In summary, this review has certain limitations which must be acknowledged when interpreting the results. It is unclear how representative of real practice the scenarios used were, and the Chi-squared analyses were limited. Therefore, the extent to which our findings are generalizable to real practice in unclear. However, our results agree with studies of real practice showing that quality of care varies, and have allowed for the generation of specific hypotheses regarding relationships between decisional components, which researchers may wish to test.

Conclusions

This review has found that in scenario studies, the appropriateness of GPs’ decision making has been assessed to a much greater extent than perceived difficulty, which is not routinely assessed. On average, 62% of the decisions made across studies were judged appropriate by some external assessment method (ranging from 81% for screening or testing decisions to only 55% for treatment or management decisions). The results also indicate that variation in difficulty might be important for decision appropriateness: specifically, greater decision difficulty may result in lower likelihood of an appropriate clinical decision being made. However, intervention studies indicate that perceived decision difficulty is modifiable and so it may be possible to improve decision appropriateness through attempts to reduce decision difficulty, although it is important to firstly establish the determinants of difficulty. Scenario-based research into GPs’ decisions rarely considers the relationship between decision difficulty and appropriateness: more research is needed to identify the specific factors which influence decision difficulty and appropriateness, to specify the relationship between decision difficulty and appropriateness, and ultimately to improve the appropriateness of clinical decisions made by GPs.

Additional files

Abbreviations

A&F:

Audit & feedback

CDSS:

Computerised decision support system

ERM:

Educational reminder messages

GP:

General Practitioner

HBOC:

Hereditary breast & ovarian cancer

HCP:

Health Care Professional

KT:

Knowledge translation

RCT:

Randomised controlled trial

References

  1. Crossing the Quality Chasm: a New Health System for the 21st Century. 2001, National Academy of Science Press, Washington, DC

  2. Anderson CJ: The psychology of doing nothing: forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychol Bull. 2003, 129 (1): 139-167. 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Hastie R: Problems for judgment and decision making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001, 52: 653-683. 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.653.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Malik MM: The effects of complexity and accountability on prescribing decision-making. PhD thesis. 1995, University of Illinois, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  5. Lewis CL, Griffith J, Pignone MP, Golin C: Physicians’ decisions about continuing or stopping colon cancer screening in the elderly: a qualitative study. J Gen Intern Med. 2009, 24 (7): 816-821. 10.1007/s11606-009-1006-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Bachmann LM, Muhleisen A, Bock A, Ter Riet G, Held U, Kessels AGH: Vignette studies of medical choice and judgement to study caregivers’ medical decision behaviour: systematic review. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2008, 8: 50-10.1186/1471-2288-8-50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, Ioannidis JP, Clarke M, Devereaux PJ, Kleijnen J, Moher D: The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. J Clin Epidemiol. 2009, 62 (10): e1-e34. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Veloski J, Tai S, Evans AS, Nash DB: Clinical vignette-based surveys: a tool for assessing physician practice variation. Am J Med Qual. 2005, 20 (3): 151-157. 10.1177/1062860605274520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sayers GM, Parera S: Withholding life prolonging treatment, and self deception. J Med Ethics. 2002, 28 (6): 347-352. 10.1136/jme.28.6.347.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Hrisos S, Eccles MP, Francis JJ, Dickinson HO, Kaner EF, Beyer F, Johnston M: Are there valid proxy measures of clinical behaviour? A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 37-10.1186/1748-5908-4-37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Higgins JPT, Green S: Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Updated Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). [.]., [www.cochrane-handbook.org]

  12. Backlund L, Danielsson B, Bring J, Strender LE: Factors influencing GPs’ decisions on the treatment of hypercholesterolaemic patients. Scand J Prim Health Care. 2000, 18 (2): 87-93. 10.1080/028134300750018963.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Tiemeier H, De Vries WJ, Van het Loo M, Kahan JP, Klazinga N, Grol R, Rigter H: Guideline adherence rates and interprofessional variation in a vignette study of depression. Qual Saf Health Care. 2002, 11 (3): 214-218. 10.1136/qhc.11.3.214.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Glazier RH, Dalby DM, Badley EM, Hawker GA, Bell MJ, Buchbinder R, Lineker SC: Management of common musculoskeletal problems: a survey of Ontario primary care physicians. CMAJ. 1998, 158 (8): 1037-1040.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Glazier RH, Dalby DM, Badley EM, Hawker GA, Bell MJ, Buchbinder R, Lineker SC: Management of the early and late presentations of rheumatoid arthritis: a survey of Ontario primary care physicians. CMAJ. 1996, 155 (6): 679-687.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Bonetti D, Eccles M, Johnston M, Steen N, Grimshaw J, Baker R, Walker A, Pitts N: Guiding the design and selection of interventions to influence the implementation of evidence-based practice: an experimental simulation of a complex intervention trial RID C-5245-2008. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60 (9): 2135-2147. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Carroll JC, Wilson BJ, Allanson J, Grimshaw J, Blaine SM, Meschino WS, Permaul JA, Graham ID: GenetiKit: a randomized controlled trial to enhance delivery of genetics services by family physicians. Fam Pract. 2011, 28 (6): 615-623. 10.1093/fampra/cmr040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Hrisos S, Eccles M, Johnston M, Francis J, Kaner EF, Steen N, Grimshaw J: An intervention modelling experiment to change GPs’ intentions to implement evidence-based practice: using theory-based interventions to promote GP management of upper respiratory tract infection without prescribing antibiotics #2. BMC Health Serv Res. 2008, 8: 10-10.1186/1472-6963-8-10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Lynggaard MD, Strandgaard S: Factors influencing the decision to start drug treatment in hypertension: a questionnaire study comparing general practitioners and hypertension specialists in Denmark. Blood Press. 2006, 15 (4): 207-212. 10.1080/08037050600913532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Short D, Frischer M, Bashford J: The development and evaluation of a computerised decision support system for primary care based upon ‘patient profile decision analysis’. Inform Prim Care. 2003, 11 (4): 195-202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Hamilton-Craig CR, Tonkin AL, Jobling RG: How accurate are hypertension treatment decisions? Absolute risk assessment and prescribing for moderate hypertension - a study of Cambridge general practitioners. Blood Press. 2000, 9 (6): 323-327. 10.1080/080370500300000897.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kostopoulou O, Oudhoff J, Nath R, Delaney BC, Munro CW, Harries C, Holder R: Predictors of diagnostic accuracy and safe management in difficult diagnostic problems in family medicine. Med Decis Making. 2008, 28 (5): 668-680. 10.1177/0272989X08319958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991, 50 (2): 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Dhami MK, Hertwig R, Hoffrage U: The role of representative design in an ecological approach to cognition. Psychol Bull. 2004, 130 (6): 959-988. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ashworth M, Armstrong D: The relationship between general practice characteristics and quality of care: a national survey of quality indicators used in the UK quality and outcomes framework, 2004–5. BMC Fam Pract. 2006, 7: 68-10.1186/1471-2296-7-68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Ashworth M, Schofield P, Seed P, Durbaba S, Kordowicz M, Jones R: Identifying poorly performing general practices in England: a longitudinal study using data from the quality and outcomes framework. J Health Serv Res Policy. 2011, 16 (1): 21-27. 10.1258/jhsrp.2010.010006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. McLean G, Sutton M, Guthrie B: Deprivation and quality of primary care services: evidence for persistence of the inverse care law from the UK Quality and Outcomes Framework. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006, 60 (11): 917-922. 10.1136/jech.2005.044628.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. McLean G, Guthrie B, Sutton M: Differences in the quality of primary medical care for CVD and diabetes across the NHS: evidence from the quality and outcomes framework. BMC Health Serv Res. 2007, 7: 74-10.1186/1472-6963-7-74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Audit Scotland: Prescribing in General Practice in Scotland. Edinburgh, Scotland, UK: 2013.

  30. Buetow SA, Sibbald B, Cantrill JA, Halliwell S: Prevalence of potentially inappropriate long term prescribing in general practice in the United Kingdom, 1980–95: systematic literature review. BMJ. 1996, 313 (7069): 1371-1374. 10.1136/bmj.313.7069.1371.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Linder JA: Antibiotic prescribing for acute respiratory infections—success that’s way off the mark: comment on “A cluster randomized trial of decision support strategies for reducing antibiotic use in acute bronchitis”. JAMA Intern Med. 2013, 173 (4): 273-275. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1984.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. NICE Clinical Guideline 69: Respiratory Tract Infections – Antibiotic Prescribing. Prescribing of Antibiotics for Self-Limiting Respiratory Tract Infections in Adults and Children in Primary Care. 2008, ᅟ, Manchester, England, UK

  33. Arnold SR, Straus SE: Interventions to improve antibiotic prescribing practices in ambulatory care. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005, 4: Art. No.: CD003539-doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003539.pub2

    Google Scholar 

  34. Hawker JI, Smith S, Smith GE, Morbey R, Johnson AP, Fleming DM, Shallcross L, Hayward AC: Trends in antibiotic prescribing in primary care for clinical syndromes subject to national recommendations to reduce antibiotic resistance, UK 1995–2011: analysis of a large database of primary care consultations. J Antimicrob Chemoth 2014. doi:10.1093/jac/dku291..

  35. Kumar S, Little P, Britten N: Why do general practitioners prescribe antibiotics for sore throat? Grounded theory interview study. BMJ. 2003, 326 (7381): 138-10.1136/bmj.326.7381.138.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Schwartz B, Mainous AG, Marcy S: Why do physicians prescribe antibiotics for children with upper respiratory tract infections?. JAMA. 1998, 279 (11): 881-882. 10.1001/jama.279.11.881.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Linder JA, Singer DE, Stafford RS: Association between antibiotic prescribing and visit duration in adults with upper respiratory tract infections. Clin Ther. 2003, 25 (9): 2419-2430. 10.1016/S0149-2918(03)80284-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Akkerman AE, Kuyvenhoven MM, van der Wouden JC, Verheij TJM: Prescribing antibiotics for respiratory tract infections by GPs: management and prescriber characteristics. Br J Gen Pract. 2005, 55 (511): 114-118.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Teixeira Rodrigues A, Roque F, Falcão A, Figueiras A, Herdeiro MT: Understanding physician antibiotic prescribing behaviour: a systematic review of qualitative studies. Int J Antimicrob Agents. 2013, 41 (3): 203-212. 10.1016/j.ijantimicag.2012.09.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Grant A, Sullivan F, Dowell J: An ethnographic exploration of influences on prescribing in general practice: why is there variation in prescribing practices?. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 72-10.1186/1748-5908-8-72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Tracy CS, Dantas GC, Moineddin R, Upshur RE: Contextual factors in clinical decision making: national survey of Canadian family physicians. Can Fam Physician. 2005, 51: 1106-1107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Brookes-Howell L, Hood K, Cooper L, Little P, Verheij T, Coenen S, Godycki-Cwirko M, Melbye H, Borras-Santos A, Worby P, Jakobsen K, Goossens H, Butler CC: Understanding variation in primary medical care: a nine-country qualitative study of clinicians’ accounts of the non-clinical factors that shape antibiotic prescribing decisions for lower respiratory tract infection. BMJ Open 2012, 2(4). doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000796..

  43. Cadieux G, Tamblyn R, Dauphinee D, Libman M: Predictors of inappropriate antibiotic prescribing among primary care physicians. CMAJ. 2007, 177 (8): 877-883. 10.1503/cmaj.070151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Coenen S, Michiels B, Renard D, Denekens J, Van Royen P: Antibiotic prescribing for acute cough: the effect of perceived patient demand. Br J Gen Pract. 2006, 56 (524): 183-190.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Kravitz RL, Bell RA, Azari R, Kelly-Reif S, Krupat E, Thom DH: Direct observation of requests for clinical services in office practice: what do patients want and do they get it?. Arch Intern Med. 2003, 163 (14): 1673-1681. 10.1001/archinte.163.14.1673.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Tonkin-Crine S, Yardley L, Little P: Antibiotic prescribing for acute respiratory tract infections in primary care: a systematic review and meta-ethnography. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2011, 66 (10): 2215-2223. 10.1093/jac/dkr279.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Grimshaw JM, Russell IT: Effect of clinical guidelines on medical practice: a systematic review of rigorous evaluations. Lancet. 1993, 342 (8883): 1317-1322. 10.1016/0140-6736(93)92244-N.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence: NICE Clinical Guideline 138: Patient Experience in Adult NHS Services: Improving the Experience of Care for People Using Adult NHS Services. Manchester, England, UK: 2012.

  49. Peabody JW, Luck J, Glassman P, Dresselhaus TR, Lee M: Comparison of vignettes, standardized patients, and chart abstraction: a prospective validation study of 3 methods for measuring quality. JAMA. 2000, 283 (13): 1715-1722. 10.1001/jama.283.13.1715.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Peabody JW, Luck J, Glassman P, Jain S, Hansen J, Spell M, Lee M: Measuring the quality of physician practice by using clinical vignettes: a prospective validation study. Ann Intern Med. 2004, 141 (10): 771-780. 10.7326/0003-4819-141-10-200411160-00008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Jones TV, Gerrity MS, Earp J: Written case simulations: do they predict physicians’ behavior?. J Clin Epidemiol. 1990, 43 (8): 805-815. 10.1016/0895-4356(90)90241-G.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Fiona Stewart for assistance with search strategy development, and with identifying relevant electronic databases and clarifying hand-searching options. We also thank Brian Power for assistance with screening titles and abstracts. We thank Phil Hannaford for reviewing the manuscript and providing useful comments. This work was supported by a Medical Research Council Doctoral Training Grant awarded to the first author. The funding source had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicola McCleary.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

NM conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination, executed the search strategy, conducted the title and abstract screening, extracted all data and performed the analysis. CR, JF, MC and JA all participated in the design and coordination of the study, participated in title and abstract screening, assisted with piloting the full-text screening and data extraction forms, and provided advice on analysis. NM wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and all other authors revised it critically. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McCleary, N., Ramsay, C.R., Francis, J.J. et al. Perceived difficulty and appropriateness of decision making by General Practitioners: a systematic review of scenario studies. BMC Health Serv Res 14, 621 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-014-0621-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-014-0621-2

Keywords