Skip to main content

Nudging healthcare professionals in clinical settings: a scoping review of the literature

Abstract

Background

Healthcare organisations are in constant need of improvement and change. Nudging has been proposed as a strategy to affect people’s choices and has been used to affect patients’ behaviour in healthcare settings. However, little is known about how nudging is being interpreted and applied to change the behaviour of healthcare professionals (HCPs). The objective of this review is to identify interventions using nudge theory to affect the behaviour of HCPs in clinical settings.

Methods

A scoping review. We searched PubMed and PsycINFO for articles published from 2010 to September 2019, including terms related to “nudging” in the title or abstract. Two reviewers screened articles for inclusion based on whether the articles described an intervention to change the behaviour of HCPs. Two reviewers extracted key information and categorized included articles. Descriptive analyses were performed on the data.

Results

Search results yielded 997 unique articles, of which 25 articles satisfied the inclusion criteria. Five additional articles were selected from the reference lists of the included articles. We identified 11 nudging strategies: accountable justification, goal setting, suggested alternatives, feedback, information transparency, peer comparison, active choice, alerts and reminders, environmental cueing/priming, defaults/pre-orders, and education. These strategies were employed to affect the following 4 target behaviours: vaccination of staff, hand hygiene, clinical procedures, prescriptions and orders. To compare approaches across so many areas, we introduced two independent dimensions to describe nudging strategies: synchronous/asynchronous, and active/passive.

Conclusion

There are relatively few studies published referring to nudge theory aimed at changing HCP behaviour in clinical settings. These studies reflect a diverse set of objectives and implement nudging strategies in a variety of ways. We suggest distinguishing active from passive nudging strategies. Passive nudging strategies may achieve the desired outcome but go unnoticed by the clinician thereby not really changing a behaviour and raising ethical concerns. Our review indicates that there are successful active strategies that engage with clinicians in a more deliberate way. However, more research is needed on how different nudging strategies impact HCP behaviour in the short and long term to improve clinical decision making.

Peer Review reports

Background

Over the last two decades, behavioural economics has gained momentum among scholars because of its innovative but also controversial ways of explaining processes and mechanisms underpinning individuals’ judgements and decision making. It was with their seminal book entitled Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness in 2008 that the behavioural economists Richard Thaler and Cass Sunstein [1] coined the concept of nudging, which they define as: “any aspect of the choice architecture that alters people’s behaviour in a predictable way without forbidding any options or significantly changing their economic incentives.” This means that any limitation of choices, such as banning or withholding information or change in incentive structures like financial rewards or taxation are not considered nudging.

Nudging quickly gained ground in several countries as a new and better method to change people’s behaviour in order to improve their health and well-being [2]. Both private and public institutions showed interest in the use of nudges because they generally cost little and have the potential to promote economic and societal goals, such as public health [3]. Since the origin of the concept in 2008, governments in the US, UK, France and many more have implemented departments of behavioural economics [4,5,6,7], sometimes called “nudge units”. Today, there are more than 200 different nudge units globally [8]. The promise of these nudge units is to use the findings of behavioural and social sciences to improve the effectiveness of government policies for modest costs and with little effort [9].

Nudging theory

In their work, Thaler and Sunstein [1] reference two modes of thinking: the automatic system and the reflective system. Cognitive scientist Daniel Kahneman refers to these as System 1 and System 2 respectively [10]. In System 1, thinking, impressions, associations, feelings, intentions, and actions flow effortlessly and quickly. We are usually in this mode when we go about our daily tasks like brushing our teeth or getting to work. In contrast, System 2 thinking is slow, effortful, and deliberate. This mode is at work when we complete a tax form or learn something new. In psychology, this is referred to as Dual Process Theory (DPT) [11].

DPT is the foundation of nudging because it explains what happens when we act unaware. At the same time, it provides us with the possibility to either make that action salient to ourselves, engaging System 2, or change the context so we choose something better without thinking about it, thus System 1 [12].

Nudging and public services

While the initial wave of nudging studies targeted mainly the adoption of healthier behaviours during exercise [13, 14], eating [15,16,17] and quitting smoking [18], nudging is now increasingly being perceived as a policy strategy to improve public services [5, 19,20,21] like the healthcare system [22].

Healthcare systems are however notorious for their complexity and conservative culture making change management rather challenging [23]. Because of its complexity, change management in healthcare requires more nuanced and well-thought-out interventions instead of top-down strategies, such as issuing more policy, prescribing more regulation, and introducing more stringent performance indicators [24]. While nudging has been increasingly discussed and used for patients and citizens, there has been less focus on the use of nudging strategies targeting healthcare professionals (HCPs).

Objectives

Our objective was to identify interventions using Thaler and Sunstein’s nudge theory to affect the behaviour of HCPs in clinical settings, focusing on target groups, nudging techniques, delivery systems and empirical evidence. In particular, we were interested in mapping which target groups have been considered, which specific nudging techniques have been used, how nudging strategies have been delivered, whether these strategies present enough empirical evidence of success, as well as the ethical implications of these strategies.

Methods

A scoping review was conducted from September 2019 to February 2020 according to the Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewers’ Manual 2017 - Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews [25].

Scoping reviews are useful for identifying knowledge gaps and synthesise available evidence and can be used to map the key concepts underpinning a research area as well as to clarify working definitions, and/or the conceptual boundaries of a topic [26]. In contrast to systematic reviews, scoping reviews provide an overview of the existing evidence, regardless of quality [25]. We therefore employed a scoping review to map out the ways in which nudging interventions have been used to affect the behaviour of healthcare professionals in clinical settings. The protocol used is explained in the following sections.

Inclusion criteria

Studies eligible for inclusion were: (i) interventions conducted in a clinical setting targeting healthcare personnel, (ii) behavioural interventions using the term nudging, (iii) randomized controlled trials, quasi-experimental or longitudinal (before-after) studies, (iii) original research articles published in English and in peer reviewed journals between 2010 and 2019.

Information sources and search strategy

A search for articles was conducted by an information specialist at the university library on two electronic databases: PubMed and PsycINFO. We searched for the term “nudging”, “nudges” or “nudge” in the title or abstract of articles published from 2010. The complete search strategy is provided as supplementary material.

Article selection

Title and abstract screenings were carried out by two reviewers with the web-based application Rayyan [27]. This screening tool allowed the authors to do individual blinded screenings of titles and abstracts. The selection of studies was undertaken in three phases of screening: (a) removing duplicates, (b) screening titles and abstract, and (c) screening full text articles. At each phase, the articles were compared against the inclusion and exclusion criteria mentioned above. In the case of the conflicting eligibility decisions a third reviewer was asked to give an additional opinion. In all these cases, consensus was finally reached. The reference list of included articles was searched for additional articles that fulfilled the inclusion criteria but that had not been found in the initial search because they did not mention “nudging” in the title nor abstract.

Data items and charting

A data-charting form was jointly developed by two reviewers to determine which variables to extract. The two reviewers independently charted the data, discussed the results, and continuously updated the data-charting form in an iterative process [28]. The form had the following sections: (a) author(s), (b) publication year, (c) study purpose/objectives, (d) study population and sample size, (e) study design, (f) effect logic, (g) type of nudge used, (h) implementation medium (e.g. physical, digital), (i) implementation details (e.g. clinic, hospital, emergency, etc), (j) geographical location, (k) study duration, (l) main outcomes and/or findings.

Results

Article selection

The database searches yielded 1024 articles as shown in Fig. 1. After removal of 27 duplicates, 972 articles were excluded on the basis of not fulfilling inclusion criteria, resulting in 25 full-text articles for inclusion. From the reference lists of these articles, 5 additional articles were identified for inclusion, resulting in a total of 30 articles for analysis. As shown in Fig. 2, the majority of included records (N = 23) were published in the last 4 y (2016–2019).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the literature search

Fig. 2
figure 2

Number of publications per year, 2010–2019

Study designs

Of the 30 included publications, the majority were prospective studies (n = 24) while the remaining six studies were retrospective. Approximately half employed a randomized control group, of which 13 were randomized controlled clinical trials and three randomized vignette-based studies, where clinicians were presented with randomly assigned hypothetical scenarios. One of the randomized controlled trials was undertaken in a simulated environment [29].

Five studies employed a control group without randomization, of which four were pre-post studies that observed a control cohort during the same period as the intervention cohort. The remaining nine articles were pre-post studies without a control cohort (see Table 1). Three of the pre-post studies employed a time-series analysis, comparing temporal trends as well as outcome differences before and after the intervention [30,31,32].

Table 1 Summary of study designs

Summary of included studies

Table 2 summarises the characteristics of the included studies. The complete data set is available in supplementary materials. Half of the studies were set across hospitals and tertiary care centres and targeted a diverse population of HCPs (physicians, nurses, midwives, medical assistants, novice health care providers, entire clinical team). Eleven studies were set in the context of ambulatory care. One study covered both primary care and specialty clinics, and one study covered an entire ambulatory healthcare system. The remaining four studies were conducted outside clinical environments: one study recruited novice healthcare providers affiliated with a medical school to conduct an RCT in a simulated setting [29], one study recruited practitioners from a collaborating hospital to undertake a vignette-based study [33], and two studies recruited practitioners across geographical regions, namely the greater Chicago area [34] and across Australia [35] to undertake vignette-based studies.

Table 2 Characteristics of the articles included in the review

Sixteen of the studies delivered their intervention via Electronic Health Record (EHR) systems, either through the prescription and ordering interface or through patient records. Six studies delivered the intervention mainly through modifications in the physical environment such as posters [36, 37], aromatization [29, 38, 39], or props [40]. Four studies combined other strategies with email communication in order to provide feedback and statistics on the performance of the target behaviour [37, 41,42,43]. One study utilized email messages to direct participants to an electronic dashboard [44]. One study delivered its intervention solely through an email message [45]. And one additional study delivered its intervention solely through a letter [46].

The two studies targeting modification of ventilation practices included the modification of default setting on the ventilation machines [30, 42] One additional study delivered a cost awareness message printed on test results [31]. Two studies delivered their interventions mainly through the modification of routine activities, namely the modification of syringe sizes for sedation during endoscopy procedures [47], and modification of inpatients’ printed charts with pain management procedures [48].

Nudging strategies and objectives

In total, 43 nudges were used in the 30 included studies, since 10 studies employed two or three nudging techniques each. From these we identified and coded 11 different nudging strategies, described in Fig. 3 and Table 3. The large majority of studies employed only one nudging strategy (n = 20). From 2016 onward, 7 studies used 2 different nudging strategies, and 3 studies included 3 nudging strategies each (See Table 2). All but one of these” multi-nudging” studies had a positive result. Of all nudges, 84% resulted in statistically positive results.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Intervention strategies over time, 2010–2019

Table 3 Description of intervention strategies

Since outcomes were very heterogeneous, we clustered articles into four different objectives to facilitate comparison. The majority of studies had as their objective to change prescription and ordering behaviour, namely, encouraging judicious antibiotic prescription [34, 36, 37, 41], increasing vaccinations orders [49, 50], increasing prescription of generic medication [51], reducing prescription of high-cost, low-value medication [32, 52], reducing unnecessary laboratory tests [33, 53], reducing imaging procedures [31, 54], increasing prescription of mouthwash to intubated patients in the intensive care unit (ICU) [55], increasing guideline-concordant prescription of statins [44], increasing prescription of blood glucose (A1C) tests for diabetes prevention [56], and increasing high-value treatment for lower-back pain [35].

The second largest target was the modification of behaviours with respect to certain care procedures (n = 7) such as ventilation settings for intubated patients [30, 42], improving inpatient sleep [49], sedation during endoscopy [47], screening for risk of cardio-vascular disease in primary care [58], sitting down during examinations [40], and pain management after Caesarean Section surgery [48]. Four studies targeted hand hygiene [29, 38, 39, 43], and two studies focused on vaccination of healthcare providers [45, 46].

Of the 17 studies with the objective of changing prescription and ordering behaviour all but two had a successful outcome (statistically significant positive results). Of the 7 studies targeting the modification of behaviours with respect to certain care procedures, only one had a non-significant result. Of the 4 studies targeting hand hygiene, three were successful. Of the 2 studies targeting vaccination of healthcare providers none had a positive result.

Quadrants

During the analysis, we found that some strategies appealing to the analytical System 2 could only cause impact if the participant chose to pay attention. For example, only showing the cost of a particular medication on the ordering screen (information transparency) can go completely unnoticed by a participant thereby not engaging the participant’s analytical mind. We also found that some strategies that automatically substituted brand medications for generics or automatically populated ordering forms (defaults/pre-orders) could also take place without the participant’s knowledge or awareness. Thus, these interventions could change the outcome of a certain behaviour without having any effect on the behaviour itself.

Another challenge with considering System 1 and System 2 categories is that they do not capture whether the intervention is presented at the time of the decision or not. For example, some interventions sent feedback emails that could be read anytime, not at the time of prescription. So, the emails are meant to change a belief that will eventually impact a behaviour, but that is very different from immediately changing a prescription to generic at the time of ordering. To capture these shortcomings, we instead developed two independent dimensions, creating four quadrants:

  • Synchronous vs. asynchronous - An intervention strategy was coded as synchronous if its delivery coincided with the decision or behaviour it intended to affect.

  • Active vs. passive - An active strategy cannot be completed without an action on the part of the participant.

Figure 4 shows the distribution of nudging objectives across the quadrants. With the exception of staff vaccination, the other nudging objectives were addressed by strategies in more than one quadrant.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Intervention objectives across quadrants

Figure 5 shows the distribution of intervention strategies across the quadrants. Three intervention strategies were assigned to more than one quadrant: alerts/reminders, defaults/pre-orders, environmental cueing/priming. Alerts and reminders often take place when the target behaviour must be performed (synchronous) and require an action from the clinician (active). Two exceptions were observed. One study was coded as active and asynchronous (Q2) [44] as it sent an email reminder prompting clinicians to access a dashboard in order to be exposed to the other nudges. The other study [46] was coded as passive and asynchronous (Q3), where letters were sent to remind front line staffers to get vaccinated, the letters could be read at any time and no action was strictly required.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Nudging strategies across quadrants

Most of the environmental cueing/priming strategies are passive and asynchronous (Q3), for example, placing posters with crafted messages and pictures. However, we coded one study [40] as synchronous because it placed chairs in examination rooms to encourage participants to sit during examination (Q4). In this case, the priming object (chair in examination room) coincides in time with the target behaviour (sitting during examination). The majority of default/pre-order interventions were synchronous and passive (Q4), for example, automatically changing a brand name medication for its generic equivalent. The” nudge” (substituting for generic) coincided in time with the target behaviour (prescribing a medication), and happened automatically even if there was no action from the clinician. There were two exceptions in this category, which implemented asynchronous strategies. One study [56] periodically populated an electronic folder with pre-approved orders for A1C laboratory tests, the other study [45] sent letters to participants with either pre-booked appointments (opt-out) for vaccination or without pre-booked appointments (opt-in).

Discussion

Since the term” nudge” was introduced already in 2008, and gained much attention as a viable intervention strategy for behavioural change, it was surprising that only 30 studies could be included in the review. However, the majority of included records were published in the last 4 y of our inclusion period. This could possibly indicate that nudging is becoming more popular in the context of behaviour interventions for HCP. Our result is in line with a new literature review addressing studies that are explicit about their use of nudge theory in influencing clinician behaviour, but with a slightly different inclusion criteria making our study complementary [59]. The authors of this review identified only 22 studies of relevance over a 10-year period (2008–2018) conducted in multiple settings and contexts.

Very few articles actually use the term “nudge” in their title or abstract, which was a necessary requirement for inclusion. Interestingly, the term “nudge” is often used in commentaries and editorials, but not in scientific articles. Rather than describing an actual nudging intervention, several articles instead discuss the potential of nudging for public policy or its ethical implications, something that has also been observed by others [59, 60].

In total, 43 nudges were used in the 30 included studies, since 10 studies employed two or three nudging techniques each. All but one of these” multi-nudging” studies had a positive result. Of all nudges, 84% resulted in statistically positive results. We included education – not traditionally seen as a nudging strategy – as a strategy since three of the” multi-nudging” articles complemented their intervention with an educational information session [40, 48, 57]. The combination of several intervention strategies, on the one hand, seems to increase the possibility that an intervention is effective, but on the other hand, makes it hard to determine exactly which elements make an intervention successful. In total, all but six studies had a positive result, which was very encouraging.

We would here like to discuss two of the target behaviours considered in our review: judicious antibiotic prescribing, and hand hygiene. All four studies targeting judicious antibiotic prescription [34, 36, 37, 41] had positive outcomes. This is an important finding since inappropriate antibiotic prescription increases cost of care, causes adverse drug reactions, and affects the growth of antibiotic-resistant bacteria [61,62,63]. Of these four interventions, two posted commitment letters in examination rooms (priming) [36, 37], two studies employed peer comparison over email messages [37, 41], and one study changed the default presentation of treatment options in an EHR [34]. One study [41] combined accountable justification with suggested alternatives via EHR by warning clinicians that “antibiotics are not recommended for this diagnosis. See alternatives below” and requiring that clinicians give a written explanation for prescribing antibiotics.

Hand hygiene is considered one of the most important measures to prevent healthcare-associated infections [64,65,66]. Three out of four of the interventions targeting hand hygiene were successful [29, 38, 39], employing priming and environmental cueing strategies. These interventions dispersed scents permeating the environment [29], displayed posters in the vicinity of gel dispensers with carefully crafted messages [39], or showed a pair of male eyes [39] in order to successfully increase hand hygiene compliance. Both the visual and olfactory cues described above were passive and asynchronous. The non-successful intervention [44] used goal setting and peer pressure instead, being active and asynchronous. But despite not achieving a significant positive result, the study showed that social cohesion – colleagues reminding each other to wash their hands – could improve hand hygiene.

Given the low cost of these priming interventions, they should be considered as viable strategies, especially in view of the recent covid-19 pandemic, when hand hygiene is a public health concern.

As noted by Sunstein [3] default rules may well be the most effective nudge, and this seems also to be the case when it comes to nudging clinicians [59]. This can be explained by status quo bias [67] and decision inertia [68], meaning that we rather keep things as they are.

One could argue, however, that creating default processes which do not require any action from the clinician may pass unnoticed. For example, an electronic prescribing system may automatically change a brand medication for its generic equivalent in a subtle way. This raises concern over infringing ethical principles to nudge someone being unaware of the nudge [2, 69,70,71,72,73]. Sunstein argues that unless active choosing is involved, some kind of default rule is essentially inevitable, regardless of whether it is intentional or not [3]. Hence, it is crucial that this type of nudge is clearly observed and that there is an opt out so that professionals can reverse the nudge to their original preference. The default studies in our review all included the possibility for the HCP to override the default, for instance prescribing another drug or changing ventilator settings. This highlights the importance of transparency, but also as suggested by Hofmann & Stanak [73] that the means of nudging also have to be in proportion to the benefit-harm ratio.

We found that, after default nudges (N = 7), active choice was the second most common strategy (N = 5) to affect prescribing behaviour. In contrast to an unnoticed default nudge, the active choice brings the choice to the forefront and requires a thoughtful action from the clinician.

Limitations

Our study has several limitations. First, we did not search other databases outside medicine and healthcare. Our rationale for this was to focus on the use of nudging strategies to affect healthcare professionals with potential effects for clinical practice. The downside of this decision is that we risk missing potentially important studies not included in PubMed and PsycINFO.

Second, we chose the terms” nudge” or” nudging” or” nudges” as our point of departure. Our intention was to find research that specifically referred to the nudging theory proposed by Thaler and Sunstein in 2008 and to better understand how it was being interpreted and employed to affect HCP in clinical settings. We chose our search terms to reflect what we believed were the most commonly known and broadly inclusive terms for describing their theory. In doing so however, we might have missed many relevant articles using related terms like “choice design” and “choice architecture”. We will include more related terms in future research.

Third, this focus on “nudging” leaves out many other interventions for quality improvement in clinical settings. We recognize that the space of possible behavioural change interventions for quality improvement in clinical settings includes many different theories other than nudging. In this article, however, we wanted to understand how nudging, as described by Thaler and Sunstein, is affecting the design of interventions for HCP.

Fourth, the heterogeneity of the articles included and the lack of consensus regarding a theoretical framework around nudging made it quite challenging for us to identify and code the relevant dimensions of the interventions. In our attempt to better categorize the interventions we introduced even newer terms that will have to be scrutinized in future work. That said, we find that the synchronous/asynchronous and active/passive dimensions provide a practical way for non-experts to reason about nudging interventions and how they may be implemented in a clinical setting.

Conclusion

The purpose of this scoping review was to identify how nudging, as described by Thaler and Sunstein, is influencing the design of interventions to affect HCPs in clinical settings. This is one of the first reviews to consider nudging as a general strategy to affect the behaviour of healthcare professionals, not limited to particular application areas nor a specific type of intervention. Despite the popularity of the term” nudging” in editorials and commentary, interventions do not often allude to nudging as defined by Thaler and Sunstein in 2008 [1]. We identified only 30 articles which mentioned terms related to” nudging” in the title or abstract and described an intervention targeting the behaviour of HCPs in a clinical setting.

When trying to assess and compare interventions with very different objectives and intervention strategies, we found that Dual Process Theory [11] or System 1 and System 2 thinking [10] did not provide a sound basis for the characterization of the interventions. It was very difficult to determine whether the nudging strategy employed was targeting System 1 or System 2, or even if the rationale for the strategy was truly having the intended effect on the clinician. For example, showing the cost of a medication can only prompt the clinician to reason about cost if he or she: (i) sees this information and (ii) decides to consider it. We therefore created two more practical dimensions to characterize nudging strategies: (a) passive/active, and (b) synchronous/asynchronous. In particular, we found that the active/passive dimension had important implications for whether the intervention was changing a behaviour long term or simply biasing a specific outcome in a given moment, and whether it infringes on any ethical principles to influence someone’s behaviour when they are not aware of the” nudge”. It is imperative that nudging is addressed in an explicit and transparent manner [73] and we therefore argue that the active/passive dimension can be used in practice to evaluate and design ethically sound nudging interventions for HCPs.

Implications for research

More research is needed on the impact of nudging healthcare providers’ attitudes and behaviours. Our study shows a large set of different nudging strategies and provides a basic yet practical categorization to explain and compare the mechanisms of different strategies. Careful ethical consideration should be given to passive strategies of which the user might not be aware. Alternatively, active strategies present fewer ethical dilemmas and are better suited for explaining the impact of nudging interventions in the behaviour of healthcare providers in the short and long term.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the article (and its additional files).

Abbreviations

A1C:

Glycated haemoglobin, or glycohemoglobin test

DPT:

Dual Process Theory

EHR:

Electronic Health Record

HCP:

Healthcare professional

ICU:

Intensive Care Unit

Q1:

Active and synchronous

Q2:

Active and asynchronous

Q3:

Passive and asynchronous

Q4:

Passive and synchronous

RCT:

Randomized Controlled Trial

References

  1. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven and London: Yale University Press; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Vallgårda S. Nudge—a new and better way to improve health? Health Policy. 2012;104(2):200–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthpol.2011.10.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Sunstein CR. Nudging: a very short guide. J Consum Policy. 2014;37(4):583–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10603-014-9273-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Obama B. Executive order—Using behavioral science insights to better serve the American people (Executive Order 13707). Washington: The White House, 2015. Retrieved from the White House; 2015. Available from: https://obamawhitehouse.archives.gov/the-press-office/2015/09/15/executive-order-usingbehavioral-science-insights-better-serve-american

  5. Halpern D. Inside the nudge unit: how small changes can make a big difference. London: WH Allen; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Dolan P, Hallsworth M, Halpern D, King D, Vlaev I. MINDSPACE: influencing behaviour for public policy. London: Institute of Government; 2010. Available from: https://www.instituteforgovernment.org.uk/publications/

    Google Scholar 

  7. Oullier O, Sauneron S. Improving public health prevention with behavioural, cognitive and neuroscience. Technical report. Paris: Centre d’analyse stratégique; 2010. Available from: https://oullier.free.fr/files/2010_Oullier-Sauneron_CAS-Neuroscience-Prevention-Public-Health_Book_Neuroeconomics-Behavioral-Economics-Neuromarketing.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  8. OECD. Behavioural insights and public policy: lessons from around the world. Paris: OEDC Publishing; 2017. Available from: https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/content/publication/9789264270480-en. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264270480-en.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  9. Thaler R. Foreword. In: Halpern D. inside the nudge unit: how small changes can make a big difference. London: WH Allen; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Strack F, Deutsch R. The duality of everyday life: Dual-process and dual system models in social psychology. In: APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, Volume 1: Attitudes and Social Cognition. American Psychological Association. 2015:891–927.

  12. iNudgeYou - The Applied Behavioural Science Group: Blog. Nudge Theory 1: The Mechanics of the Brain; 2012. Available from: https://inudgeyou.com/en/nudge-theory-1-the-mechanics-of-the-brain/ Accessed 7 Oct 2020.

  13. Forberger S, Reisch L, Kampfmann T, Zeeb H. Nudging to move: a scoping review of the use of choice architecture interventions to promote physical activity in the general population. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1):77. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-019-0844-z.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Ledderer L, Kjær M, Madsen EK, Busch J, Fage-Butler A. Nudging in public health lifestyle interventions: a systematic literature review and metasynthesis. Health Educ Behav. 2020;47(5):749–64. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198120931788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Wilson AL, Buckley E, Buckley JD, Bogomolova S. Nudging healthier food and beverage choices through salience and priming. Evidence from a systematic review. Food Qual Prefer. 2016;51:47–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.02.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Tørris C, Mobekk H. Improving cardiovascular health through nudging healthier food choices: a systematic review. Nutrients. 2019;11(10):2520. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102520.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Bucher T, Collins C, Rollo ME, McCaffrey TA, De Vlieger N, Van der Bend D, et al. Nudging consumers towards healthier choices: a systematic review of positional influences on food choice. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(12):2252–63. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114516001653.

  18. Sunstein CR. Nudging smokers. N Engl J Med. 2015;372(22):2150–1. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMe1503200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. French R, Oreopoulos P. Applying behavioural economics to public policy in Canada. Can J Econ. 2017;50(3):599–635. https://doi.org/10.1111/caje.12272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Araña JE, León CJ. Can defaults save the climate? Evidence from a field experiment on carbon offsetting programs. Environ Resour Econ. 2013;54(4):613–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10640-012-9615-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Bache I, Reardon L, Anand P. Wellbeing as a wicked problem: navigating the arguments for the role of government. J Happiness Stud. 2016;17(3):893–912. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9623-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Patel MS, Volpp KG, Asch DA. Nudge units to improve the delivery of health care. N Engl J Med. 2018;378(3):214–6. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1712984.

  23. Wensing M, Grol R, Grimshaw J. Improving patient care: the implementation of change in health care. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; 2020. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119488620.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  24. Braithwaite J. Changing how we think about healthcare improvement. BMJ. 2018;361:k2014. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.k2014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Peters MD, Godfrey CM, Khalil H, McInerney P, Parker D, Soares CB. Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):141–6. https://doi.org/10.1097/XEB.0000000000000050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Arksey H, O’Malley L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. 2005;8(1):19–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, Elmagarmid A. Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev. 2016;5(1):210. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Lenzen SA, Daniëls R, van Bokhoven MA, van der Weijden T, Beurskens A. Disentangling self-management goal setting and action planning: a scoping review. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0188822. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188822.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Birnbach DJ, King D, Vlaev I, Rosen LF, Harvey PD. Impact of environmental olfactory cues on hand hygiene behaviour in a simulated hospital environment: a randomized study. J Hosp Infect. 2013;85(1):79–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhin.2013.06.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Bourdeaux CP, Thomas MJ, Gould TH, Malhotra G, Jarvstad A, Jones T, et al. Increasing compliance with low tidal volume ventilation in the ICU with two nudge-based interventions: evaluation through intervention time-series analyses. BMJ Open. 2016;6(5):e010129. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010129.

  31. Lewis S, Young B, Thurley P, Shaw D, Cranwell J, Skelly R, et al. Evaluation of a nudge intervention providing simple feedback to clinicians of the consequence of radiation exposure on demand for computed tomography: a controlled study. Clin Med. 2019;19(4):290–3. https://doi.org/10.7861/clinmedicine.19-4-290.

  32. Langley T, Lacey J, Johnson A, Newman C, Subramanian D, Khare M, et al. An evaluation of a price transparency intervention for two commonly prescribed medications on total institutional expenditure: a prospective study. Future Healthc J. 2018;5(3):198–202. https://doi.org/10.7861/futurehosp.5-3-198.

  33. Probst CA, Shaffer VA, Chan YR. The effect of defaults in an electronic health record on laboratory test ordering practices for pediatric patients. Health Psychol. 2013;32(9):995–1002. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Tannenbaum D, Doctor JN, Persell SD, Friedberg MW, Meeker D, Friesema EM, et al. Nudging physician prescription decisions by partitioning the order set: results of a vignette-based study. J Gen Intern Med. 2015;30(3):298–304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-014-3051-2.

  35. Soon J, Traeger AC, Elshaug AG, Cvejic E, Maher CG, Doust JA, et al. Effect of two behavioural ‘nudging’ interventions on management decisions for low back pain: a randomised vignette-based study in general practitioners. BMJ Qual Saf. 2019;28(7):547–55. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2018-008659.

  36. Meeker D, Knight TK, Friedberg MW, Linder JA, Goldstein NJ, Fox CR, et al. Nudging guideline-concordant antibiotic prescribing: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Inter Med. 2014;174(3):425–31. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.14191.

  37. Yadav K, Meeker D, Mistry RD, Doctor JN, Fleming-Dutra KE, Fleischman RJ, et al. A multifaceted intervention improves prescribing for acute respiratory infection for adults and children in emergency department and urgent care settings. Acad Emerg Med. 2019;26(7):719–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/acem.13690.

  38. Caris MG, Labuschagne HA, Dekker M, Kramer MHH, van Agtmael MA, Vandenbroucke-Grauls CMJE. Nudging to improve hand hygiene. J Hosp Infect. 2018;98(4):352–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhin.2017.09.023.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. King D, Vlaev I, Everett-Thomas R, Fitzpatrick M, Darzi A, Birnbach DJ. “Priming” hand hygiene compliance in clinical environments. Health Psychol. 2016;35(1):96–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Orloski CJ, Tabakin ER, Shofer FS, Myers JS, Mills AM. Grab a seat! Nudging providers to sit improves the patient experience in the emergency department. J Pat Exp. 2019;6(2):110–6. https://doi.org/10.1177/2374373518778862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Meeker D, Linder JA, Fox CR, Friedberg MW, Persell SD, Goldstein NJ, et al. Effect of behavioral interventions on inappropriate antibiotic prescribing among primary care practices: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2016;315(6):562–70. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.0275.

  42. O’Reilly-Shah VN, Easton GS, Jabaley CS, Lynde GC. Variable effectiveness of stepwise implementation of nudge-type interventions to improve provider compliance with intraoperative low tidal volume ventilation. BMJ Qual Saf. 2018;27(12):1008–18. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2017-007684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kwok YLA, Harris P, McLaws M-L. Social cohesion: the missing factor required for a successful hand hygiene program. Am J Infect Control. 2017;45(3):222–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2016.10.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Patel MS, Kurtzman GW, Kannan S, Small DS, Morris A, Honeywell S Jr, et al. Effect of an automated patient dashboard using active choice and peer comparison performance feedback to physicians on statin prescribing: the PRESCRIBE cluster randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2018;1(3):180818.

  45. Lehmann BA, Chapman GB, Franssen FM, Kok G, Ruiter RA. Changing the default to promote influenza vaccination among health care workers. Vaccine. 2016;34(11):1389–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2016.01.046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Schmidtke KA, Nightingale PG, Reeves K, Gallier S, Vlaev I, Watson SI, et al. Randomised controlled trial of a theory-based intervention to prompt front-line staff to take up the seasonal influenza vaccine. BMJ Qual Saf. 2020;29(3):189–97. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2019-009775.

  47. Harewood GC, Clancy K, Engela J, Abdulrahim M, Lohan K, O’Reilly C. Randomised clinical trial: a ‘nudge’ strategy to modify endoscopic sedation practice. Aliment Pharmacol Ther. 2011;34(2):229–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2036.2011.04703.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Shakespeare T, Fehlberg M, Slejko T, Taylor J, Srbinovska I, Bolsin S. Successful use of “choice architecture” and “nudge theory” in a quality improvement initiative of analgesia administration after caesarean section. J Eval Clin Pract. 2019;25(1):125–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/jep.13037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Arora VM, Machado N, Anderson SL, Desai N, Marsack W, Blossomgame S, et al. Effectiveness of SIESTA on objective and subjective metrics of nighttime hospital sleep disruptors. J Hosp Med. 2019;14(1):38–41. https://doi.org/10.12788/jhm.3091.

  50. Kim RH, Day SC, Small DS, Snider CK, Rareshide CAL, Patel MS. Variations in influenza vaccination by clinic appointment time and an active choice intervention in the electronic health record to increase influenza vaccination. JAMA Netw Open. 2018;1(5):e181770. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.1770.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Patel MS, Volpp KG, Small DS, Wynne C, Zhu J, Yang L, et al. Using active choice within the electronic health record to increase influenza vaccination rates. J Gen Intern Med. 2017;32(7):790–5. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-017-4046-6.

  52. Malhotra S, Cheriff AD, Gossey JT, Cole CL, Kaushal R, Ancker JS. Effects of an e-prescribing interface redesign on rates of generic drug prescribing: exploiting default options. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2016;23(5):891–8. https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocv192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Monsen CB, Liao JM, Gaster B, Flynn KJ, Payne TH. The effect of medication cost transparency alerts on prescriber behavior. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2019;26(10):920–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocz025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Sedrak MS, Myers JS, Small DS, Nachamkin I, Ziemba JB, Murray D, et al. Effect of a Price transparency intervention in the electronic health record on clinician ordering of inpatient laboratory tests: the PRICE randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(7):939–45. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.1144.

  55. Sharma S, Guttmann D, Small DS, Rareshide CAL, Jones J, Patel MS, et al. Effect of introducing a default order in the electronic medical record on unnecessary daily imaging during palliative radiotherapy for adults with Cancer: a stepped-wedge cluster randomized clinical trial. JAMA Oncol. 2019;5(8):1220–2. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaoncol.2019.1432.

  56. Bourdeaux CP, Davies KJ, Thomas MJ, Bewley JS, Gould TH. Using 'nudge' principles for order set design: a before and after evaluation of an electronic prescribing template in critical care. BMJ Qual Saf. 2014;23(5):382–8. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2013-002395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Panattoni L, Chan A, Yang Y, Olson C, Tai-Seale M. Nudging physicians and patients with autopend clinical decision support to improve diabetes management. Am J Manag Care. 2018;24(10):479–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Holt TA, Thorogood M, Griffiths F, Munday S, Friede T, Stables D. Automated electronic reminders to facilitate primary cardiovascular disease prevention: randomised controlled trial. Br J Gen Pract. 2010;60(573):e137–43. https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgp10X483904.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Lamprell K, Tran Y, Arnolda G, Braithwaite J. Nudging clinicians: a systematic scoping review of the literature. J Eval Clin Pract. 2021 Feb;27(1):175–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/jep.13401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Hummel D, Maedche A. How effective is nudging? A quantitative review on the effect sizes and limits of empirical nudging studies. J Behav Exp Econ. 2019;80:47–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2019.03.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Antibiotic Resistance Threats in the United States, 2019. Atlanta: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, CDC; 2019. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/drugresistance/pdf/threats-report/2019-ar-threats-report-508.pdf

    Book  Google Scholar 

  62. European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control. Proposals for EU guidelines on the prudent use of antimicrobials in humans. Stockholm: ECDC; 2017. Available from: https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/sites/portal/files/media/en/publications/Publications/EU-guidelines-prudent-use-antimicrobials.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  63. World Health Organization. Global action plan on antimicrobial resistance. Geneva: WHO; 2015. Available from: https://www.who.int/antimicrobial-resistance/global-action-plan/en/

    Google Scholar 

  64. Pittet D. Compliance with hand disinfection and its impact on hospital-acquired infections. J Hosp Infect. 2001;48(Suppl A):S40–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Larson E. A causal link between handwashing and risk of infection? Examination of the evidence. Infect Control. 1988;9(1):28–36.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Group HL. Hand washing: a modest measure—with big effects. BMJ. 1999;318(7185):686. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.318.7185.686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Samuelson W, Zeckhauser R. Status quo bias in decision making. J Risk Uncertain. 1988;1(1):7–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00055564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Alós-Ferrer C, Hügelschäfer S, Li J. Inertia and decision making. Front Psychol. 2016;7:169.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Reisch LA, Sunstein CR. Do Europeans like nudges? Judgm Decis Mak. 2016;11(4):310–25.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Sunstein CR. People prefer system 2 nudges (kind of). Duke L J. 2016;66 Available from: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2731868.

  71. Hagman W, Andersson D, Västfjäll D, Tinghög G. Public views on policies involving nudges. Rev Philos Psychol. 2015;6(3):439–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-015-0263-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Jung JY, Mellers BA. American attitudes toward nudges. Judgm Decis Mak. 2016;11(1):62–74.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Hofmann B, Stanak M. Nudging in screening: literature review and ethical guidance. Patient Educ Couns. 2018;101(9):1561–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2018.03.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Authors would especially like to thank the information specialists form University of Gothenburg University Library (Gothenburg, Sweden) for help with the literature search.

Funding

The study was funded by the Centre for Person-Centred Care (GPCC). GPCC is funded by the Swedish Government’s grant for Strategic Research Areas, Care Sciences (Application to Swedish Research Council No. 2009–1088) and cofounded by the University of Gothenburg. Open Access funding provided by University of Gothenburg.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AS and AV performed the main analysis and wrote the manuscript. AW supervised all the steps in the analysis and contributed with the manuscript writing. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Axel Wolf.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sant’Anna, A., Vilhelmsson, A. & Wolf, A. Nudging healthcare professionals in clinical settings: a scoping review of the literature. BMC Health Serv Res 21, 543 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-021-06496-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-021-06496-z

Keywords