Skip to main content

Managing expectations: cognitive authority and experienced control in complex healthcare processes

Abstract

Background

Balancing the normative expectations of others (accountabilities) against the personal and distributed resources available to meet them (capacity) is a ubiquitous feature of social relations in many settings. This is an important problem in the management of long–term conditions, because of widespread problems of non-adherence to treatment regimens. Using long-term conditions as an example, we set out middle range theory of this balancing work.

Methods

A middle-range theory was constructed four stages. First, a qualitative elicitation study of men with heart failure was used to develop general propositions about patient and care giver experience, and about the ways that the organisation and delivery of care affected this. Second, these propositions were developed and confirmed through a systematic review of qualitative research literature. Third, theoretical propositions and constructs were built, refined and presented as a logic model associated with two main theoretical propositions. Finally, a construct validation exercise was undertaken, in which construct definitions informed reanalysis of a set of systematic reviews of studies of patient and caregiver experiences of heart failure that had been included in an earlier meta-review.

Results

Cognitive Authority Theory identifies, characterises and explains negotiation processes in in which people manage their relations with the expectations of normative systems – like those encountered in the management of long-term conditions. Here, their cognitive authority is the product of an assessment of competence, trustworthiness and credibility made about a person by other participants in a healthcare process; and their experienced control is a function of the degree to which they successfully manage the external process-specific limiting factors that make it difficult to otherwise perform in their role.

Conclusion

Cognitive Authority Theory assists in explaining how participants in complex social processes manage important relational aspects of inequalities in power and expertise. It can play an important part in understanding the dynamics of participation in healthcare processes. It suggests ways in which these burdens may lead to relationally induced non-adherence to treatment regimens and self-care programmes, and points to targets where intervention may reduce these adverse outcomes.

Peer Review reports

Background

Negotiating the expectations of others is a routine part of our everyday lives. It involves us in balancing the things that they hold us accountable for (their normative expectations of us), with our ability to deliver on them (our capacity to act). Most readers of this article will have experienced such negotiations. For example, they are at the centre of the practices of appraisal and performance review that take place in universities and other corporations; they run through interactions between patients and healthcare professionals as they examine adherence to treatment regimens; and they dominate relations between the managers and owners of football teams. Our aim in this paper is to offer a middle range theory—Cognitive Authority Theory—that will facilitate understanding of the relational mechanisms involved in balancing out capacity and accountability.

As a worked example of the theory, we use the problem of balancing capacity and accountability in chronic disease management in the community. The growing epidemiological crisis over long-term (chronic) conditions, and the demographic crisis represented by an increasing proportion of older people with multiple long-term conditions [1], means that clinicians and policy makers increasingly emphasise self-care or self-management as a strategy for patient care. Here, the work of problem recognition and management, and the challenge of ongoing implementation of physical activity and changes to diet; adherence to monitoring and treatment regimens; and responding to the administrative demands of healthcare systems, are explicitly shifted to the patient and caregiver. This has led to the recognition that people experience not just the burden of symptoms, but also burden of treatment. These new burdens are a problem for people who must do the work that stems from this, often in the context of intrusive and debilitating symptoms and their psychosocial consequences [2,3,4]. Systematic reviews of studies of patient experience consistently reveal that this work is complex and demanding. They also reveal that the combined burdens of symptom and burdens of treatment are sometimes very hard to bear, and that they can overwhelm people with long-term conditions, with the potential for poor health outcomes as a result [5,6,7,8,9,10]. These poor outcomes may come about when patient and caregiver capacity is exceeded by the self-care workload that is delegated to them by healthcare systems and for which they are accountable [3].

The idea that adherence to treatment regimens and self-care programmes is work does not fit well with how we understand and evaluate engagement and participation in them. The most commonly used psychological theory in this context, is Bandura’s theory of self-efficacy. It is ‘is concerned with judgments of how well one can execute [the] courses of action required to deal with prospective situations’ [11] (p.122), and focuses on individuals’ beliefs and self-appraisals of their competence in performing specific tasks [12]. The evaluation of self-care interventions often focuses on measuring subjective self-efficacy, and associates improvements in self-efficacy with improvements in self-care. The basic proposition that underpins this kind of approach is that increased confidence in ability to perform a specific set of actions converts into sustained motivation to adhere to a structured regime of activities. This is certainly true in the short-term, and associations with a series of positive health outcomes have commonly been reported [13,14,15,16]. However, this picture is complicated because investigations tend to focus on index conditions rather than multi-morbidities, which are associated with lower levels of self-efficacy [17]. In addition, reviews of self-management programmes have detected only small to moderate increases in self-efficacy and selected health outcomes beyond 12 months [18, 19]. The reliance on self-efficacy as a primary outcome in studies of self-care and self-management programmes has meant less attention has been paid to the role of contextual and environmental mechanisms and resources in shaping patient behaviour [20]. Against this background, patient and caregiver experiences of the complexity of care need to be understood both in terms of the work that they do [4, 21,22,23], and of the ways that healthcare providers call on them to do it [24]. Interventions to improve self-care, to enhance treatment adherence, and to promote shared decision-making in the clinical encounter all depend on these encounters. Recently, researchers in this field have focused on the development of measures of treatment burden [25,26,27,28,29], but we also need to understand the situational and relational factors that shape it.

Our aim in this paper is to set out a theory of the behavioural and social mechanisms through which people balance capacity and accountability in relation to a set of social roles and processes. The theory responds to an important problem in the behavioural sciences: how can we best understand the dynamics of human agency under conditions of constraint [30]? This is the fundamental problem of structure and agency that runs through contemporary sociology. In this paper, we are not concerned with those elevated debates. Instead, we use self-care regimens for long-term conditions to illuminate the ways that capacity and accountability are balanced out, and show how the theory helps us to understand mechanisms that affect the sustainability of healthcare interventions for people with these conditions. It need not be restricted to these groups and these settings, however. It can be applied to any setting where inequalities of power exist. Clinical setting offer well-characterised examples of domains in which individuals negotiate their personal capacity for action whilst also interacting with institutional accountabilities and organisational expectations. Cognitive Authority Theory also contributes to understanding experiences of Burden of Treatment by identifying and characterising the factors that shape the interactional work required to adhere to self-care and other management regimens, and to access and use healthcare services. In turn, this helps us to see how patient and caregiver capacity are not fixed. Instead, they arise through negotiations about obligations.

Methods

In this paper, we present Cognitive Authority Theory: which is a middle-range theory of the negotiation of normative expectations. It is a middle range theory because it focuses on a limited set of propositions or assumptions [31], and it is a theory and not a model or framework because these assumptions can be applied independently of context [32]. There are no uniformly accepted methods for building theory. Indeed there are diverse and conflicting ideas about what theories are [33]. In the work presented in this paper, we followed the methods successfully used previously by one of us to develop and refine theories in medical sociology, health services research and implementation science [2, 34, 35]. This comprises four sets of tasks.

  1. a.

    Identification of sensitising concepts. First, KH undertook a small-scale elicitation study of older men’s (single phase of semi-structured interviews, n = 10) experiences of medical care for heart failure and comorbidities [36]. This small-scale exploratory qualitative study identified a set of behaviours related to participants’ interactions with health professionals and health care provision. This work was extended through a systematic review by Demain et al., [6] that explored how patients sought to manage, and attempted to minimise, the impact of treatments on their lives in order to maintain control. This formed a set of sensitising concepts. Research Ethics Committee approval for the elicitation study described in this paper was granted by NHS (England) South Central Research Ethics Committee on 21 December 2012: REC reference: 12/SC/0638.

  2. b.

    Characterisation of theoretical constructs . Next, we integrated sensitising concepts derived from the elictiation study and Demain et al.’s., [6], systematic review with key elements of the growing body of theoretical and empirical literature on Normalization Processes, experiences of Cumulative Complexity, and Burden of Treatment [2, 4, 7, 29, 37,38,39], applied to long-term conditions.

  3. c.

    Modelling constructs . We then sifted and sorted integrated concepts, writing them as propositions that expressed generic constructs about behaviour, we linked the results of taxonomy building described above to constructs derived from already existing theories relevant to illness burden and treatment burden. This taxonomy focused attention on the relational processes within a healthcare system. We continued this until we reached the most parsimonious possible model of interactions between constructs. We then mapped the relationships between constructs, as shown in Fig. 1. Definitions of these constructs are provided in Table 1.

  4. d.

    Construct validation. The final stage of theory building was to develop a set of context-independent propositions about these relations. We then linked these together in a summary statement of the theory. This summary statement characterised and explained assumptions about the role of cognitive authority and experienced control in relational processes within a social system. We used these propositions and their related concepts as a coding framework for attribution analysis of 20 reports of patient and caregiver experiences of heart failure [40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60] that had been collected for an earlier overview of systematic reviews of qualitative studies by May et al., [10, 61]. These studies are described in Table 2., and simple counts [62] of attributions coded to the constructs of the theory are shown in Fig. 2. In Table 3, we show how sensitising concepts, theoretical constructs, and coded examples from the literature review are linked together.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A context independent model of cognitive authority and experienced control

Table 1 Definitions of key constructs of the theory
Table 2 Qualitative systematic reviews experiences of Chronic Heart Failure coded in construct validation (extracted from May et al. [10, 60])
Fig. 2
figure 2

Simple counts of construct attributions derived in the construct validation phase

Table 3 Sensitising concepts, theory constructs and examples from the literature

Results: Cognitive authority and experienced control

Cognitive Authority Theory assists in understanding the ways that people negotiate complex relational processes in conditions marked by unequal social relations. We developed the theory through two assumptions that suggest how (less powerful) patients and caregivers interact with (more powerful) healthcare providers and organisations. First, we are concerned with the cognitive authority of actors in a complex social process. Cognitive authority itself is the product of an assessment of competence, trustworthiness and credibility made about a person by other participants in a process.

  1. (i)

    The degree of cognitive authority possessed by an actor in a complex social process depends on balancing their available capacity against the expectations of others.

Second, we are concerned with the extent to which a person assesses their obligations within their role as ‘doable’ in the context of their life-world and experience; and the degree to which they successfully manage the external process-specific limiting factors that make it difficult to otherwise perform in their role. That is, the extent of their experienced control.

  1. (ii)

    Experienced control over a process depends on participants’ ability to negotiate and manage process-specific limiting factors that challenge their capacity to achieve goals.

In what follows, we unpack these two assumptions: defining accountabilities, capacity, and process-specific limiting factors, and then developing the concepts of cognitive authority and experienced control. Because our theory is context-independent, we distinguish between two general classes of participants in these relational processes. These are institutional actors (who can be considered more powerful by virtue of the authority derived from their institutional position, and who include healthcare providers), and population actors (who can be considered less powerful because they are expected to defer to institutional actors, and include patients and caregivers).

To connect these constructs with real-world processes, we focus on aspects of patient and caregiver experiences of long-term life-limiting conditions. We draw on recent studies of experiences of such conditions through revisiting a meta-review of systematic reviews of qualitative studies of patient experiences of heart failure, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, and chronic kidney failure [10, 61]. Interactions between patients, caregivers, healthcare professionals, policy-makers, and services are an excellent vehicle for understanding participation in complex relational processes.

Patient goals and illness careers

The purpose of Cognitive Authority Theory is to help us make sense of the relationships between actors, as they negotiate a process, within a dynamic social system. By social system, we mean that they are in an organised pattern of social relations in which participants have broad awareness of the rules and resources that inform behaviour within it, have broad awareness of the goals of others within it, and share relative agreement about the appropriate ways of achieving those goals. Taking this approach enables us to deal with the problem of agency under structural constraints, noted at the beginning of the paper. Fligstein has drawn on interactionist perspectives to characterise these behaviours as ‘social skill’ [63].

Social skill can be defined as the ability to induce cooperation among others. Skilled social actors empathetically relate to the situations of other people and, in doing so, are able to provide those people with reasons to cooperate (…). Skilled social actors must understand how the sets of actors in their group view their multiple conceptions of interest and identity and how those in external groups do as well. They use this understanding in particular situations to provide an interpretation of the situation and frame courses of action that appeal to existing interests and identities (p.112).

Within such systems, we are interested in social processes characterised by negotiations about compliance and obligations within a set of normative expectations of action, experienced under conditions of constraint. In these negotiations, participants relate these expectations to (i) individual and distributed capacity to participate, and (ii) the contexts in which such action takes place and which present participants with process-specific limiting factors. When they do so, it is often within the context of a temporal process defined by status passage [64].

A status passage occurs when individuals, or groups, experience institutionally and organisationally ascribed transitions over time. In these transitions, identities are redefined, and interactions and relations with others are reconfigured. Following from this come changed normative expectations about their roles and actions and, in turn, changes in the ways that others respond to them. Status passages exemplify a change in state over time—the most fundamental definition of a process—and they rely on shared definitions of the legitimacy of role changes, and shared understandings of the nature of the underlying reconfiguration of identity.

In long-term conditions, reviews demonstrate consistent evidence to the effects of such changes in experienced identity: these may include reduced self-esteem and self-worth, and loss of social functioning [49, 58, 65,66,67,68,69]. The effects of these changes can be powerful, and they include fear, anxiety, isolation and discomfort [48, 50, 55, 58, 70, 71]. Awareness of the meaning of disease and it implications frame the ways that people interact with services [43, 47, 67, 72, 73]. At the same time, those anxieties could be ameliorated by finding ways to improve the ways that patients and caregivers could influence the course and direction of their passage through care [71, 72, 74, 75].

Unequal negotiations between population and institutional actors

We have already observed that participants in relational processes can be treated as though they are members of two groups. Here, more powerful actors possess institutional or organisational qualities that empower them to impose accountabilities and normative expectations of others. These expectations have institutional or organisational sources; they are generalised across groups or populations though implicit theories that characterise the identities and explain the behaviours of less powerful actors. Against this background, the possibilities for the exercise of social skill (i.e. negotiating towards goals), by population actors depends on the degree of cognitive authority that they can negotiate with institutional ones. We define cognitive authority as the extent to which population actors are seen by others to possess qualities of competence, trustworthiness and credibility in meeting the accountabilities and accomplishing the tasks implicated in a relational process. Cognitive authority forms an important resource for actors when these are involved in negotiations about their participation in an institutionally defined process. It rests on the extent to which more powerful actors accept the experiential claims that less powerful actors make about their state and how this affects their available capacity to participate in that process and thus meet their accountabilities. By capacity, we mean the extent to which less powerful actors can access and mobilize individual and group resources. These resources may have affective, cognitive, informational, material, physical and relational components.

Studies of the lived experience of chronic or long-term conditions consistently show how interactions between these two classes of actors are formed and worked out in real-world settings. Patients and caregivers live with disease processes that are governed by biological mechanisms of pathophysiological deterioration, and in which they experience and make sense of symptoms and effects of disease, and the effects of treatments [76]. These processes are given meaning by social mechanisms of status passage, in which they experience and make sense of changes in social identity and status that are attributed to them by others [64]. Health system responses to these diseases aim to retard, or at least stabilise, disease progression. They promote health-related behaviour change such as diet and exercise [77]; weight loss [56]; smoking cessation [78]. They also enrol patients and caregivers into a set of delegated activities (symptom recognition and monitoring [79]; medication adherence and management [51]; participation in rehabilitation programmes [70]; and the operation of health technologies [80]. The normative assumption that underpins these interventions is that the patient – and caregiver – will be motivated to engage in behaviour change and to participate in delegated clinical activities.

Internationally, there is now a mass of policy that stresses that motivated and adaptive participation in self-care is a key element of control over disease progression [81]. Patient and caregiver capacity is the necessary foundation for this [82]. While empirical studies do much to reveal the ways in which normative expectations of participation are played out, they also show how patient and caregiver capacity is formed and expended. Indeed, caregivers’ solidarity seems to be central to this process [40, 53, 65, 83]. It supports symptom recognition and effective engagement with services [53, 55, 84, 85], but diminishes when caregiver workload interferes with normal life [40, 41, 74, 75, 80, 86].

Process limiting factors

Cognitive authority is the product of negotiations about agency—available capacity and its exercise—between population and institutional actors. However, these relations are often situated in contexts where neither party may be able to control the course or direction of a process, and where capacity and accountability are constrained by the intervention of external mechanisms that affect them. External mechanisms take the form of process-specific limiting factors when they intervene in ways that change participants’ actions within a relational process. Actors must develop and negotiate coping strategies to successfully overcome those external factors and experience control over the effects of what may be the result of interactions between irreversible pathophysiological processes (e.g. disease progression and its effects), and large-scale structural mechanisms (e.g. resource allocation and organisation in healthcare systems). These define the rate of change in a relational process, and direct the course of important events within it. Here, patient and caregiver capacity is equally diminished by their access to services and other resources [70, 79, 86, 87], and by poor professional support, co-ordination and responsiveness, and continuity of care [44, 51, 68, 70, 79, 86]. Indeed, patients and formal and informal caregivers are often disadvantaged by poorly communicated information about disease processes and symptoms [45, 55, 67, 72, 88, 89]. Although pathophysiological and cognitive deterioration matter a great deal [90], it is often clinician behaviours that matter more. For example, these may include reluctance to discuss the life-limiting nature of heart failure and to inform patients and caregivers about the meaning of symptoms [43].

Relations between population and institutional actors that underpin negotiations that inform experienced control over a relational process are rarely equal. One is empowered to define the ways in which the other exercises its capacity, and to hold it accountable for what it does. Unstable and unequal relations are fertile ground for complex interactions between system regulators and system subjects, and they are framed by a set of power relations through which rules that govern behaviour [91], and the roles and relative positions of participants [92], are defined and enacted. In the relations on which this paper focuses—those between population and institutional actors participating in self-care regimes—there is a substantial body of theory already that characterises the relative powers of patients and clinicians through medical dominance theory [93, 94]; identity theory [95]; and status passage theory [96]. Patients and caregivers cannot negotiate with healthcare professionals who are reluctant to communicate openly about their condition [43, 97, 98], or who impute accountabilities to them that they have no capacity to meet [99].

Discussion

We have outlined Cognitive Authority Theory: a middle-range theory that describes and explains mechanisms through which actors seek to exercise social skill within a status passage. Here cognitive authority and experienced control moderate population actors’ experiences of both their accountabilities (the normative expectations of institutional actors), and process-specific limiting factors (other relational and structural mechanisms that intrude into and externally shape status passage). We mapped the constructs of the theory in Fig. 1. To illustrate how the theory works, we have drawn on the wider literature that examines patient experiences of life-limiting long-term conditions, and on qualitative data collected in a small-scale elicitation study. In Fig. 3, we mapped the constructs of the theory directly on to those experiences. In this form, our theory helps to bridge the gap between three higher-level theories: Bandura’s social psychological theory of agency [12] and self-efficacy [11]; Fligstein’s theory of social skill as inducing co-operation in dynamic social systems [63]; and Glaser and Strauss’ theory of status passages as dynamic processes of identity formation and negotiation [64].

Fig. 3
figure 3

Cognitive authority: complex interactions between experienced status passage and consequences of clinical encounters

Summary statement of the theory

In summary, Cognitive Authority Theory rests on the following assumptions.

  1. (a)

    The degree to which population actors implicated in a complex social process possess Cognitive Authority depends on them balancing their available capacity against the expectations of others. Their experienced control over a situation or process depends on their ability to negotiate and manage process-specific limiting factors that challenge their capacity to achieve goals.

  2. (b)

    Cognitive Authority and Experienced Control are mediated by actors’ capacity, defined as the extent to which an actor possesses and can mobilize personal resources, (which may be affective, cognitive, informational, material, physical and relational), in the service of those goals; and by their accountability, defined as the extent to which others impute to the actor the ability to mobilize those resources to meet their expectations.

  3. (c)

    Capacity and accountability are moderated by interaction processes that lead to negotiated obligations. These negotiated obligations may be temporary, but they provide a normative structure for future action because they define the degree of accountability that can be imputed to an actor.

These assumptions lead the theory to propose that:

  1. (d)

    When population actors in a complex relational process are faced with normative expectations of institutional actors that exceed their perceived capacity, they will seek opportunities to negotiate the relational balance between capacity and accountability.

  2. (e)

    In relation to (d), the theory proposes that the cognitive authority possessed by a population actor depends on their degree of success in balancing their available capacity against the accountabilities imputed to them by institutional actors; and that the degree of sustained motivation to meet negotiated accountabilities is proportionate to the cognitive authority possessed by population actors.

  3. (f)

    When population actors in a complex relational process are faced with demands of process-specific limiting factors that exceed their perceived capacity, they will seek opportunities to negotiate the practical balance between capacity and practical demands.

  4. (g)

    In relation to (f), the theory proposes that the degree of experienced control possessed by a population actor depends on their degree of success in balancing their available capacity against the constraining effects of the contexts in which these tasks must be undertaken; and that the degree of sustained motivation to meet negotiated accountabilities is proportionate to population actors’ experienced control over process-specific limiting factors.

The problem of relationally induced non-adherence

Given the empirical focus of our account of Cognitive Authority Theory, it is not surprising that one important area for its application is in understanding the management of long-term or chronic conditions. As we observed earlier in the paper, research framed by Normalization Process Theory [39], the Cumulative Complexity Model [4], and Burden of Treatment Theory [2], has focused on understanding the relationship between the workload that healthcare providers ask patients to take on, and the patient’s capacity to do this work. This has led to the notion that the diminution of patient participation in self-care programmes and adherence to treatment regimens over time may be structurally induced [3], when patients and caregivers become overburdened by the demands of care. Put simply, workload becomes too much to manage. Cognitive Authority Theory suggests that there is another dimension to patient experience that is not fully accounted for in either the Cumulative Complexity Model or Burden of Treatment Theory. This is that the mechanisms that lead to diminution in participation in self-care programmes and treatment regimens over time have an important relational component.

The concept of relationally induced non-adherence is potentially important. Like structurally induced non-adherence, it is iatrogenic. It may arise when institutional actors either cannot or will not enter into negotiations over population actors’ beliefs or knowledge about what is possible within the context of an experienced process. In healthcare, this means that the patient’s claim to cognitive authority is denied, (and it is not far from this to a deficit model of patient beliefs and behaviours), and their attempts to exercise experienced control over process-limiting factors may be complicated by paternalistic models of care.

Patients, caregivers and health professionals vary in their need and desire for authority and control [100] and research has shown that agreement on healthcare responsibilities between patients and health professionals is associated with better health outcomes [101]. We propose that the negotiation of doable tasks and reasonable obligations positively influences cognitive authority and experienced control. Clinical tools already exist that can be applied to the problem of relationally induced non-adherence to treatment regimens and programme participation. These include high quality shared decision-making tools [102, 103], and consultation models of deliberative and collaborative encounters between patients, caregivers, and healthcare providers [104].

Conclusion

The theory of Cognitive Authority presented in this paper helps us understand key mechanisms involved in configuring people’s experiences of long-term life limiting conditions. Although we have applied it to experiences of long-term conditions, it is context-independent in its fundamentals, and can be applied to unequally formed relations between population (less powerful) and institutional (more powerful) actors in a wide variety of situations. In this paper, we have shown how the theory can be used to explore the relational content of patient work in the management of long-term conditions. This has important implications for healthcare provider organisations, for whom structurally or relationally induced non-adherence is a potential problem. It highlights the importance of implementing shared decision-making techniques in the clinical encounter in ways that enable clinicians, patients and caregivers to focus on achievable and sustainable objectives. Beyond this, it provides a way of showing the effects of normative expectations expressed by healthcare providers and policy-makers. When these fit poorly with patients’ and caregivers’ actual capacity for participation, and when they fail to take into account the effects of system level limiting mechanisms, then non-adherence to treatment regimens can be relationally induced. Better understanding of the processes and mechanisms characterised in this paper will lead to ways to improve support for patients and healthcare professionals in making help-seeking behaviours more appropriately focused, and self-care regimens more sustainable.

We have focused on the ways that Cognitive Authority Theory explains important aspects on relations between people with long-term conditions and clinicians. There is no reason, however, to restrict it to this. At the beginning of this paper, we made the point that negotiating normative expectations is ubiquitous in social relations. The approach we have presented here can be readily applied to relations between health professionals, between patients and caregivers—or between participants in any set of organised social relations where inequalities of power are negotiated. Cognitive Authority Theory explains (a) mechanisms that shape experienced control over process-specific limiting factors, within a process characterised by status passage, and (b) the role of the cognitive authority of participants in motivating and shaping that process. Future empirical research will refine its constructs and test its predictions

References

  1. Gallacher KI, Batty GD, McLean G, Mercer SW, Guthrie B, May CR, et al. Stroke, multimorbidity and polypharmacy in a nationally representative sample of 1,424,378 patients in Scotland: implications for treatment burden. BMC Med. 2014;12(1):151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. May C, Eton DT, Boehmer KR, Gallacher K, Hunt K, MacDonald S, et al. Rethinking the patient: using burden of treatment theory to understand the changing dynamics of illness. BMC Health Serv Res. 2014;14(1):281.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. May C, Montori VM, Mair FS. We need minimally disruptive medicine. BMJ. 2009;339(aug11_2):b2803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Shippee ND, Shah ND, May CR, Mair FS, Montori VM. Cumulative complexity: a functional, patient-centered model of patient complexity can improve research and practice. J Clin Epidemiol. 2012;65(10):1041–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Boger E, Ellis J, Latter S, Foster C, Kennedy A, Jones F, et al. Self-management and self-management support outcomes: a systematic review and mixed research synthesis of stakeholder views. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):e0130990.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Demain S, Gonçalves A-C, Areia C, Oliveira R, Marcos AJ, Marques A, et al. Living with, managing and Minimising treatment burden in long term conditions: a systematic review of qualitative research. PLoS One. 2015;10(5):e0125457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Gallacher K, Morrison D, Jani B, Macdonald S, May CR, Montori VM, et al. Uncovering treatment burden as a key concept for stroke care: a systematic review of qualitative research. PLoS Med. 2013;10(6):e1001473.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Sav A, King MA, Whitty JA, Kendall E, McMillan SS, Kelly F, Hunter B, Wheeler AJ. Burden of treatment for chronic illness: a concept analysis and review of the literature. Health Expect. 2015;18(3):312–24.

  9. Boehmer KR, Gionfriddo MR, Rodriguez-Gutierrez R, Dabrh AMA, Leppin AL, Hargraves I, et al. Patient capacity and constraints in the experience of chronic disease: a qualitative systematic review and thematic synthesis. BMC Fam Pract. 2016;17(1):1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. May CR, Cummings A, Myall M, Harvey J, Pope C, Griffith P, et al. Experiences of long-term lifelimiting conditions among patients and carers: what can we learn from a meta-review of systematic reviews of qualitative studies of chronic heart failure, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and chronic kidney disease? BMJ Open. 2016;6:e011694.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Bandura A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am Psychol. 1982;37(2):122–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84(2):191–215.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lev EL, Daley KM, Conner NE, Reith M, Fernandez C, Owen SV. An intervention to increase quality of life and self-care self-efficacy and decrease symptoms in breast cancer patients. Res Theory Nurs Pract. 2001;15(3):277–94.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Cramm JM, Strating MM, Roebroeck ME, Nieboer AP. The importance of general self-efficacy for the quality of life of adolescents with chronic conditions. Soc Indic Res. 2013;113(1):551–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Gong G, Mao J. Health-related quality of life among Chinese patients with rheumatoid arthritis: the predictive roles of fatigue, functional disability, self-efficacy, and social support. Nurs Res. 2016;65(1):55–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Simpson E, Jones M. An exploration of self-efficacy and self-management in COPD patients. Brit J Nurs. 2013;22(19):1105–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Dickson VV, Buck H, Riegel B. Multiple comorbid conditions challenge heart failure self-care by decreasing self-efficacy. Nurs Res. 2013;62(1):2–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Brady TJ, Murphy L, O'Colmain BJ, Beauchesne D, Daniels B, Greenberg M. A meta-analysis of health status, health behaviors, and health care utilization outcomes of the chronic disease self-management program. Preventing Chronic Dis. 2013;10:E07.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Newbould J, Taylor D, Bury M. Lay-led self-management in chronic illness: a review of the evidence. Chronic Illn. 2006;2(4):249–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Fox NJ, Ward KJ, O’Rourke AJ. The ‘expert patient’: empowerment or medical dominance? The case of weight loss, pharmaceutical drugs and the internet. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60(6):1299–309.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Strauss A, Schatzman L, Bucher R, Ehrlichman D, Sabshin M. Psychiatric ideologies and institutions. New York: Free Press; 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Strauss AL. Negotiations: Varieties, contexts, processes, and social order. San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass Inc Pub; 1978.

  23. Pickard S, Rogers A. Knowing as practice: self-care in the case of chronic multi-morbidities. Social Theory & Health. 2012;10(2):101–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Boehmer KR, Shippee ND, Beebe TJ, Montori VM. Pursuing minimally disruptive medicine: correlation of patient capacity with disruption from illness and healthcare-related demands. J Clin Epidemiol. 2016;74:227–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Tran V-T, Harrington M, Montori VM, Barnes C, Wicks P, Ravaud P. Adaptation and validation of the treatment burden questionnaire (TBQ) in English using an internet platform. BMC Med. 2014;12(1):109.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Tran VT, Montori VM, Eton DT, Baruch D, Falissard B, Ravaud P. Development and description of measurement properties of an instrument to assess treatment burden among patients with multiple chronic conditions. BMC Med. 2012;10(1):68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Eton DT, de Oliveira DR, Egginton JS, Ridgeway JL, Odell L, May CR, et al. Building a measurement framework of burden of treatment in complex patients with chronic conditions: a qualitative study. Patient Rel Outcome Meas. 2012;3:39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Eton DT, Yost KJ, Lai J-s, Ridgeway JL, Egginton JS, Rosedahl JK, et al. Development and validation of the patient experience with treatment and self-management (PETS): a patient-reported measure of treatment burden. Qual Life Res. 2017;26:489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Sav A, Whitty JA, McMillan SS, Kendall E, Kelly F, King MA, Wheeler AJ. Treatment Burden and Chronic Illness: Who is at Most Risk? Patient. 2016;9:559.

  30. May CR, Johnson M, Finch T. Implementation, context and complexity. Implement Sci. 2016;11(1):141.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Merton RK. Social theory and social structure. New York: Free Press; 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Boudon R. What middle-range theories are. Contemp Sociol. 1991;20(4):519–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Swedberg R. Theorizing in sociology and social science: turning to the context of discovery. Theor Soc. 2012;41(1):1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. May C. A rational model for assessing and evaluating complex interventions in health care. BMC Health Serv Res. 2006;6(86):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  35. May CR, Mair F, Finch T, MacFarlane A, Dowrick C, Treweek S, et al. Development of a theory of implementation and integration: normalization process theory. Implement Sci. 2009;4:29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Hunt K, James EP, Anton-Solanas I, Masters J, Richardson A, May CR. Bystander behaviour and control: hpw healthcare organisation and hierarchies challenge the work of patienthood in the management of illness complexity. Unpublished Working Paper, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Southampton 2015.

  37. May C: Retheorizing the clinical encounter. In: Assaults on the Lifeworld: new directions in the sociology of chronic and disabling conditions. Edited by Scambler G, Scambler S. London Routledge; 2010.

  38. Eton DT, Ridgeway JL, Egginton JS, Tiedje K, Linzer M, Boehm DH, et al. Finalizing a measurement framework for the burden of treatment in complex patients with chronic conditions. Patient Relat Outcome Meas. 2015;6:117–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Gallacher K, May CR, Montori VM, Mair FS. Understanding patients’ experiences of treatment burden in chronic heart failure using normalization process theory. Ann Fam Med. 2011;9(3):235–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Molloy GJ, Johnston DW, Witham MD. Family caregiving and congestive heart failure. Review and analysis. Eur J Heart Fail. 2005;7(4):592–603.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Yu DS, Lee DT, Kwong AN, Thompson DR, Woo J. Living with chronic heart failure: a review of qualitative studies of older people. J Adv Nurs. 2008;61(5):474–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hopp FP, Thornton N, Martin L. The lived experience of heart failure at the end of life: a systematic literature review. Health Soc Work. 2010;35(2):109–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Barclay S, Momen N, Case-Upton S, Kuhn I, Smith E. End-of-life care conversations with heart failure patients: a systematic literature review and narrative synthesis. Br J Gen Pract. 2011;61(582):e49–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Dev S, Abernethy AP, Rogers JG, O'Connor CM. Preferences of people with advanced heart failure-a structured narrative literature review to inform decision making in the palliative care setting. Am Heart J. 2012;164(3):313–319.e315.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Dickson VV, Buck H, Riegel B. A qualitative meta-analysis of heart failure self-care practices among individuals with multiple comorbid conditions. J Card Fail. 2011;17(5):413–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Kang X, Li Z, Nolan MT. Informal caregivers’ experiences of caring for patients with chronic heart failure: systematic review and metasynthesis of qualitative studies. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2011;26(5):386–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Low J, Pattenden J, Candy B, Beattie JM, Jones L. Palliative care in advanced heart failure: an international review of the perspectives of recipients and health professionals on care provision. J Card Fail. 2011;17(3):231–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Tierney S, Mamas M, Skelton D, Woods S, Rutter MK, Gibson M, et al. What can we learn from patients with heart failure about exercise adherence? A systematic review of qualitative papers. Health Psychol. 2011;30(4):401–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Thomas JR, Clark AM. Women with heart failure are at high psychosocial risk: A systematic review of how sex and gender influence heart failure self-care. Cardiol Res Pract. 2011; 1(1).

  50. Clark AM, Savard LA, Spaling MA, Heath S, Duncan AS, Spiers JA. Understanding help-seeking decisions in people with heart failure: a qualitative systematic review. Int J Nurs Stud. 2012;49(12):1582–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Jani B, Blane D, Browne S, Montori V, May C, Shippee N, et al. Identifying treatment burden as an important concept for end of life care in those with advanced heart failure. Curr Opinion Support Palliat Care. 2013;7(1):3–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Procter E. Collaboration between the specialties in provision of end-of-life care for all in the UK: reality or utopia? Int J Palliat Nurs. 2012;18(7):339–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Buck HG, Harkness K, Wion R, Carroll SL, Cosman T, Kaasalainen S, et al. Caregivers’ contributions to heart failure self-care: a systematic review. Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2015;14(1):79–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Falk H, Ekman I, Anderson R, Fu M, Granger B. Older patients’ experiences of heart failure-an integrative literature review. J Nurs Schol. 2013;45(3):247–55.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Siabani S, Leeder SR, Davidson PM. Barriers and facilitators to self-care in chronic heart failure: a meta-synthesis of qualitative studies. Spring. 2013;2:320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Sookhoo D, Pellowe C, Derham C. The experiences of heart failure patients following their participation in self-management patient education programmes: a systematic review. JBI Database of Systematic Reviews and Implementation Reports. 2013;11(2):236–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Clark AM, Spaling M, Harkness K, Spiers J, Strachan PH, Thompson DR, Currie K. Determinants of effective heart failure self-care: a systematic review of patients’ and caregivers’ perceptions. Heart. 2014;100:716–21.

  58. Dekker RL. Patient perspectives about depressive symptoms in heart failure: a review of the qualitative literature. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2014;29(1):E9–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Strachan PH, Currie K, Harkness K, Spaling M, Clark AM. Context matters in heart failure self-care: a qualitative systematic review. J Card Fail. 2014;20(6):448–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Wingham J, Harding G, Britten N, Dalal H. Heart failure patients’ attitudes, beliefs, expectations and experiences of self-management strategies: a qualitative synthesis. Chronic Illn. 2014;10(2):135–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. May CR, Masters J, Welch L, Hunt K, Pope C, Myall M, et al. EXPERTS 1—experiences of long-term life-limiting conditions among patients and carers: protocol for a qualitative meta-synthesis and conceptual modelling study. BMJ Open. 2015;5(4):e007372.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Hsieh H-F, Shannon SE. Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res. 2005;15(9):1277–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Fligstein N. Social skill and the theory of fields. Sociol Theor. 2001;19(2):105–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Glaser BG, Strauss A. Status passage. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd; 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Tong A, Cheung KL, Nair SS, Kurella Tamura M, Craig JC, Winkelmayer WC. Thematic synthesis of qualitative studies on patient and caregiver perspectives on end-of-life care in CKD. Am J Kidney Dis. 2014;63(6):913–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Wadd K, King L, Bennett P, Grant J. Being a parent on dialysis: a literature review. J. 2011;37(4):208–15.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Welstand J, Carson A, Rutherford P. Living with heart failure: an integrative review. Int J Nurs Stud. 2009;46(10):1374–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Jeon YH, Kraus SG, Jowsey T, Glasgow NJ. The experience of living with chronic heart failure: a narrative review of qualitative studies. BMC Health Serv Res. 2010;10:77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Rolls TP, Young LE. Disrupting the biomedical discourse: older Women's lived experiences with heart failure: a feminist review of the literature. Canadian Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing. 2012;22(1):18–25.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Thorpe O, Johnston K, Kumar S. Barriers and enablers to physical activity participation in patients with COPD: a systematic review. J Cardiopulm Rehabil Prevent. 2012;32(6):359–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Makaroff KL. Experiences of kidney failure: a qualitative meta-synthesis. Nephrol Nurs J. 2012;39(1):21–9. 80; quiz 30

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Moustakas J, Bennett PN, Nicholson J, Tranter S. The needs of older people with advanced chronic kidney disease choosing supportive care: a review. Renal Society of Australasia Journal. 2012;8(2):70–5.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Disler RT, Green A, Luckett T, Newton PJ, Inglis S, Currow DC, et al. Experience of advanced chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: metasynthesis of qualitative research. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2014;48(6):1182–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Harwood L, Clark AM. Understanding pre-dialysis modality decision-making: a meta-synthesis of qualitative studies. Int J Nurs Stud. 2013;50(1):109–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Low J, Smith G, Burns A, Jones L. The impact of end-stage kidney disease (ESKD) on close persons: a literature review. NDT Plus. 2008;1(2):67–79.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Sullivan M. The new subjective medicine: taking the patient's point of view on health care and health. Soc Sci Med. 2003;56(7):1595–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Palmer SC, Hanson CS, Craig JC, Strippoli GF, Ruospo M, Campbell K, et al. Dietary and fluid restrictions in CKD: a thematic synthesis of patient views from qualitative studies. Am J Kidney Dis. 2015;65(4):559–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Kirkpatrick P, Wilson E, Wimpenny P. Support for older people with COPD in community settings: a systematic review of qualitative research. JBI Database of Systematic Reviews and Implementation Reports. 2012;10(57):3649–763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. McEntee ML, Cuomo LR, Dennison CR. Patient-, provider-, and system-level barriers to heart failure care. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2009;24(4):290–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Walker RC, Hanson CS, Palmer SC, Howard K, Morton RL, Marshall MR, et al. Patient and caregiver perspectives on home hemodialysis: a systematic review. Am J Kidney Dis. 2015;65(3):451–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Morgan HM, Entwistle VA, Cribb A, Christmas S, Owens J, Skea ZC, et al. We need to talk about purpose: a critical interpretive synthesis of health and social care professionals’ approaches to self-management support for people with long-term conditions. Health Expect. 2017;20(2):243–259. doi: 10.1111/hex.12453.

  82. Huber M, van Vliet M, Giezenberg M, Winkens B, Heerkens Y, Dagnelie PC, et al. Towards a ‘patient-centred’ operationalisation of the new dynamic concept of health: a mixed methods study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(1):e010091.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Langer S, Chew-Graham C, Hunter C, Guthrie EA, Salmon P. Why do patients with long-term conditions use unscheduled care? A qualitative literature review. HealthSoc Care Comm. 2013;21(4):339–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Disler RT, Gallagher RD, Davidson PM. Factors influencing self-management in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: an integrative review. Int J Nurs Stud. 2012;49(2):230–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Cullen DL, Stiffler D. Long-term oxygen therapy: review from the patientsg perspective. Chronic Resp Dis. 2009;6(3):141–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Oishi A, Murtagh FE. The challenges of uncertainty and interprofessional collaboration in palliative care for non-cancer patients in the community: a systematic review of views from patients, carers and health-care professionals. Palliat Med. 2014;28(9):1081–98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. de Sousa Pinto JM, Martin-Nogueras AM, Morano MT, Macedo TE, Arenillas JI, Troosters T. Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease patients’ experience with pulmonary rehabilitation: a systematic review of qualitative research. Chronic Resp Dis. 2013;10(3):141–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Gysels M, Bausewein C, Higginson IJ. Experiences of breathlessness: a systematic review of the qualitative literature. Palliat Support Care. 2007;5(3):281–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Giacomini M, DeJean D, Simeonov D, Smith A. Experiences of living and dying with COPD: a systematic review and synthesis of the qualitative empirical literature. Ont Health Technol Assess Ser. 2012;12(13):1–47.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Kessing D, Denollet J, Widdershoven J, Kupper N. Psychological determinants of heart failure self-care: systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychosom Med. 2016;78(4):412–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Therborn G. Back to norms! On the scope and dynamics of norms and normative action. Curr Sociol. 2003;50(6):863–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Turner JH. A theory of social interaction. Cambridge: Polity Press; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Kemp R. Medical dominance and institutional change in the delivery of health care services. For Social Econ. 2007;36:43–51.

  94. Freidson E. The profession of medicine. Chicago: Chicago University Press; 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Armstrong D. A new history of identity: a sociology of medical knowledge. Basingstoke: Palgrave; 2002.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  96. Glaser BG, Strauss AL. Awareness of dying. New York: Transaction; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  97. Momen N, Hadfield P, Kuhn I, Smith E, Barclay S. Discussing an uncertain future: end-of-life care conversations in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. A systematic literature review and narrative synthesis. Thorax. 2012;67(9):777–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Boyd K, Mason B, Kendall M, Barclay S, Chinn D, Thomas K, et al. Advance care planning for cancer patients in primary care: a feasibility study. Brit J Gen Pract. 2010;60(581):e449–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Bohlen K, Scoville E, Shippee ND, May CR, Montori VM. Overwhelmed patients a videographic analysis of how patients with type 2 diabetes and clinicians articulate and address treatment burden during clinical encounters. Diabetes Care. 2012;35(1):47–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Street RL, Krupat E, Bell RA, Kravitz RL, Haidet P. Beliefs about control in the physician-patient relationship. J Gen Int Med. 2003;18(8):609–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Auerbach SM, Clore JN, Kiesler DJ, Orr T, Pegg PO, Quick BG, et al. Relation of diabetic patients’ health-related control appraisals and physician–patient interpersonal impacts to patients’ metabolic control and satisfaction with treatment. J Behav Med. 2002;25(1):17–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Montori VM, Gafni A, Charles C. A shared treatment decision-making approach between patients with chronic conditions and their clinicians: the case of diabetes. Health Expect. 2006;9(1):25–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Lloyd A, Joseph-Williams N, Edwards A, Rix A, Elwyn G. Patchy ‘coherence’: using normalization process theory to evaluate a multi-faceted shared decision making implementation program (MAGIC). Implement Sci. 2013;8:102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Elwyn G, Lloyd A, May C, van der Weijden T, Stiggelbout A, Edwards A, et al. Collaborative deliberation: A model for patient care. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;97(2):158–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Dedication . We mourn the loss of Peter Cripps, whose experiences inspired much of the work leading to KJH’s contribution to this paper. What we know of his final days and hours exemplifies the brutal incivilities that follow from professional rejection of the cognitive authority of patients and caregivers, and institutionalised indifference to their attempts to gain some experienced control over the processes of their care. We wish it had not been so.

Key ideas developed in this paper have been presented in seminars of the Complex Healthcare Processes Research Group, University of Southampton, UK (February 2016), and the Institute of Health Sciences, University of Stavanger, Norway (October 2016). We are grateful to participants in those meetings, and to our collaborators in the work leading up to this paper. We thank members of the International Working Group on Minimally Disruptive Medicine, Kasey Boehmer, David Eton, Katie Gallacher, Frances Mair, Victor M Montori, and Nathan Shippee. Along with Anne Sophie Darlington, Claire Foster, Chloe Grimmett, Christine May, Alison Richardson and Theresa Wiseman, they commented very generously on the ideas presented here.

Funding

KJH and CRM were supported by the National Institute for Health Research Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (NIHR CLAHRC) Wessex, which is a partnership between Wessex NHS organizations and partners and the University of Southampton. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. Funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Please contact KJH.

Authors’ contributions

KJH and CRM contributed equally to the work and are joint guarantors of the paper. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Katherine Hunt is a post-doctoral Faculty Research Fellow and Carl May is Professor of Healthcare Innovation in the Complex Healthcare Processes Research Group in the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Southampton.

Competing interests

CRM is a member of the international editorial advisory board of BMC Health Services Research. The authors declare that they have no other competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Research Ethics Committee approval for the elicitation study described in this paper was granted by NHS (England) South Central Research Ethics Committee on 21 December 2012: REC reference: 12/SC/0638. Written consent to join the study was obtained from all participants.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carl R. May.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hunt, K.J., May, C.R. Managing expectations: cognitive authority and experienced control in complex healthcare processes. BMC Health Serv Res 17, 459 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-017-2366-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-017-2366-1

Keywords