Skip to main content
  • Systematic Review
  • Open access
  • Published:

The effectiveness of visual-based interventions on health literacy in health care: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Abstract

Background

Limited Health Literacy (HL) is an obstacle to accessing and receiving optimal health care and negatively impacts patients’ quality of life, thus making it an urgent issue in the health care system. Visual-based interventions are a promising strategy to improve HL through the use of visual aids and pictorial materials to explain health-related concepts. However, a comprehensive summary of the literature on the topic is still scarce.

Methods

To fill this gap, we carried out a systematic review and meta-analysis with the aim to determine the effectiveness of visual-based interventions in improving comprehension of health related material in the clinical population. Independent studies evaluating the effectiveness of visual-based interventions on adults (> 18 years) and whose primary outcome was either health literacy (HL) or comprehension were eligible for the review. After a systematic literature search was carried out in five databases, 28 studies met the inclusion criteria and thus were included. Most of the studies were randomized controlled trials and they focused on HL and health knowledge as outcomes.

Results

The review and meta-analysis showed that visual-based interventions were most effective in enhancing the comprehension of health-related material compared to traditional methods. According to meta-analytic results, videos are more effective than traditional methods (Z = 5.45, 95% CI [0.35, 0.75], p < 0.00001) and than the employment of written material (Z = 7.59, 95% CI [0.48, 0.82], p < 0.00001). Despite this, no significant difference was found between video and oral discussion (Z = 1.70, 95% CI [-0.46, 0.53], p = 0.09).

Conclusions

We conclude that visual-based interventions, particularly the ones using videos, are effective for improving HL and the comprehension of health-related material.

Peer Review reports

Background

Health literacy (HL) is defined as “the degree to which individuals have the capacity to obtain, process, and understand basic health information and services needed to make appropriate health decisions” [1]. Limited HL has been described as an obstacle to accessing healthcare [2] and it negatively impacts the patient’s quality of life and health outcomes. Hence, it represents an urgent issue in the health care system. Prevalence of low HL levels are still high in Europe where 33% to 50% of people cannot comprehend basic health information [3]. This percentage increases in the United States, where only 12% of people shows a proficient level of HL [4, 5]. On the other hand, the prevalence of limited HL in Southeast Asian countries floats from 35.1% to 75.6%, with an average of 55.3% [6]. Limited HL negatively affects the healthcare system as a whole, including the use of resources and the economic burden. For example, limited HL is associated with non-adherence to pre-operative guidance and leads to detrimental health behaviors, including unhealthy lifestyles, drug abuse, and tobacco smoking [7, 8]. Berkman and colleagues [9] summarized the results of 96 studies and reported that limited HL is consistently associated with higher hospitalization rates, indiscriminate usage of the emergency department, low medication adherence, and failure to use preventive medicine services (i.e., cancer screening and vaccines). Limited HL often results in the misuse of resources and, consequently, wastes economic assets. For example, in the systematic review by Eichler and colleagues [10], summarizing ten studies, the costs associated with limited HL represented 3–5% of the overall expenditure covered by the healthcare systems in Switzerland and the United States. According to the authors, an individual with poor HL incurs an additional health expense ranging between $143 and $7.798 each year compared to a person with adequate HL. Hence, due to the high prevalence of limited HL in the population and its impact on societies and healthcare costs at large, there is an increasing need to address this gap by using more efficient methods to adapt medical materials to patients’ needs and abilities and, hence, to improve their knowledge.

In this regard, visual-based interventions are encouraging approaches to address limited HL due to their applicability and promising results [11]. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention defines these interventions as “images, videos, and similar tools used to communicate information about a specific topic and to simplify the comprehension process” [12]. The advantages of using visual-based tools have already been documented in 1971, when Allan Paivio elaborated the Dual Coding Theory [13, 14] according to which the double encoding procedures reinforce the stimulus generated by visual cues. Images are encoded via multiple pathways, as they are simultaneously processed by the sensory and the verbal systems. In contrast, written words are only encoded by the verbal system. Since multiple systems are involved in elaborating visual materials, the “picture superiority effect” has been a key in facilitating comprehension and understanding [13, 14]. A qualitative synthesis performed in 2006 revealed that pictorial aids effectively enhanced patients’ comprehension of medications and treatments [15].

Furthermore, a systematic review of 52 studies reported significant improvements in understanding health-related materials among patients provided with audio-visual cues compared to standard methods like written information sheets or oral discussions with physicians [16]. However, this latter review focused on a broad range of interventions rather than visual-based ones. Furthermore, Lee and colleagues [17] reported that using icons, color codes, and larger font sizes improved health outcomes regarding hypertension, heart failure, and hypercholesterolemia among patients with limited HL. More recently, a scoping review was mapped the existing studies on digital video interventions for mental health literacy among young people, underlying the importance of studying these interventions and the initial stage in mapping the literature [18].

Although the literature provides a consistent number of independent studies on the effectiveness of visual-based intervention in improving the comprehension of health-related materials, systematic reviews on this topic are still scarce, while a meta-analysis is entirely lacking. Hence, the present study aims to systematically and meta-analytically summarize the scientific literature on the effectiveness of visual-based interventions in improving HL and the comprehension of health-related information in the adult population. Based on this contextual information, EG, LM and PS, formulated the research question in PICO format: In an adult population (P), composed of either healthy or not impaired individuals (P), what is the effect of the usage of visual-based intervention (I) in improving comprehension of health related materials (O) compared to standard methods of information delivery (C), through a systematic review and metanalysis investigating the effectiveness of these intervention (S). In doing so, we hypothesized that (i) visual-based interventions would effectively improve HL and the comprehension of health-related materials among adult patients. Also, we expect that (ii) patients who receive health information through more interactive material (e.g., videos) would report higher levels of comprehension than standard methods of information delivery (e.g., oral discussion or written information).

Methods

The systematic review and meta-analysis was performed according to current PRISMA guidelines [19]. EG, LM and PS determined the most appropriate databases to conduct the literature search. Various databases were evaluated in order to ensure comprehensive coverage of relevant literature. As a result, five databases were used to perform a literature search on March 22nd: Behavioral Science Collection, CINHAL, Communication and Mass Media Complete, MEDLINE, and PsychInfo. EG and LM developed the list of keywords tailored to the research objectives. PS revised and approved them ensuring alignment with the study’s aim. The search terms covered two key topics: “visual intervention” and “health literacy”. The Boolean Operators “OR” and “AND” were used to divide keyterms within and between the two categories, respectively (see Table S1 in the Appendix for the complete list of keywords). Additionally, EG carried out a handsearch in Google Scholar on April 14th by entering selected keywords and reviewing the first 100 entries of each combination. EG performed the selection procedure between March 23rd and April 20th.

Eligibility criteria

Studies were included in the systematic review and meta-analysis according to inclusion criteria previously defined by the authors. In particular, a study has to (1) be written in English and published in a peer-reviewed journal, (2) focus on visual-based intervention within the health-care setting, (3) include an adult population (≥ 18 years), (4) include either healthy or individuals from a clinical setting, and (5) use a measure of HL or comprehension of health related material as the outcome. No restriction was applied to the visual type or to the disease or healthcare area under investigation. Additionally, studies reporting a result convertible into an effect size were further included in the meta-analysis. A study was excluded if: (1) it investigated other interventions than visual aids, (2) the study population was composed of individuals under 18 years, (3) the study population consisted of adults on behalf of minors or impaired individuals, (4) HL or comprehension was not explicitly stated as outcomes, and (5) if the language differed from English. Duplicates, dissertations, books, magazines, reviews, editorial material, letters, and retracted publications were also excluded.

Data collection

For each included publication, EG extrapolated the following information: title, author(s)’s name, year of publication, health-care area (that was further divided into health promotion, health prevention, disease management, and consent or risk management), disease under investigation, study design, geographical location, description of the setting, the final number of participants, sample characteristics (i.e., age, gender, ethnic group), type of visual-based intervention, comparators such as standard methods of information delivery (written information sheet, oral discussion with doctor), outcome measure, the definition of HL (if present), and a brief description of the results.

Risk of bias assessment

The quality of the included studies was evaluated according to a selection of items from the Cochrane Risk of Bias assessment (RoB2) [20]. In particular, each article was evaluated for the procedure under which subjects were assigned to study arms (selection bias), the extent to which participants and researchers were aware of the allocation process (performance bias), the management of missing data (attrition bias) and the methods employed for measuring the outcome (detection bias) [20]. A single coder (EG) performed this assessment on Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5.4.1).

Data analysis

The meta-analysis was performed on RevMan 5.4.1. EG, LM, and PS contributed to the analysis and interpretation of data. The team used a standardized mean difference to calculate the effect size. Cut-off levels of Hedge’s g were set at 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 for small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively [21]. The inconsistency index (I2) was used to measure heterogeneity across studies [21]. In order to interpret the results of the I2, the following standard cut-off values were set: an I2 comprised between 0 and 40% (low) represented no significant difference across studies, whereas an I2 greater than 75% indicated considerable heterogeneity; the interval in between was interpreted as either moderate or substantial differences between studies [21].

Results

Characteristics of included studies

The literature search in the databases and hand search returned 3060 results. Out of these, 442 were duplicates, dissertations, books, and reviews and hence were excluded. Then, titles and abstracts of 2592 entries were screened, ending in 68 records eligible for full-text screening. Throughout the screening of titles and abstracts, a significant number of studies were deemed ineligible for inclusion. The predominant reasons for exclusion were the lack of measurement of the outcome of our interest, namely the comprehension of health related material and, more frequently, a predominant focus on ophthalmologic conditions or pathologies related to the visual system, a bias likely introduced by specific keywords within the search string pertaining to the domain of visual interventions. Among these, 28 studies met the inclusion criteria and were then included in the systematic review. Out of these, ten studies reported data convertible into effect sizes and were then included in the meta-analysis. The selection process is represented in the PRISMA flowchart (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flowchart

Description and design of included studies

The current systematic review is based on 28 studies [11, 22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48]. The characteristics of the included studies are reported in the Appendix (Table S2).

The total number of participants included in the studies was 5347, with sample sizes of individual studies ranging from 31 to 821 participants (Fig. S1). Included studies reported an equal distribution in terms of males (53.36%) and females (45.46%), whereas one study did not report gender information [45] (Fig. S2). The mean age of participants in the included studies was 53.8 ± 12.1 years, ranging from a minimum of 33.7 to a maximum of 74 years (Figs. S3 and S4). Only few studies provided information regarding participants’ educational level (n = 19) or ethnic group (n = 9). Among these, 50.7% had less than a high school degree, whereas 22.25% and 27.04% had a high school diploma or more, respectively (Fig. S5). Information regarding the ethnic group was available for 1451 participants, among which 50.52% were Caucasian, 17.44% were Hispanic, 25.78% were African-American, and 0.83% were Asian. The remaining 5.44% represented study participants for whom the ethnic group differed from those mentioned above (Fig. S6).

All the studies were randomized clinical trials (RCTs) except for three. Among the RCTs, two adopted a randomized cross-over study design [32, 48]. Whereas, within non-RCTs, two adopted a quasi-experimental design [29, 40] and one a pre-post study design [25] (Fig. S7).

The majority of the studies (n = 14) were conducted in North America (U.S. and Canada) and Oceania (n = 6). In contrast, fewer studies were conducted in Europe (n = 4), Asia (n = 3), and Africa (n = 1) (Figs. S8 and S9). The effectiveness of the visual-based intervention was evaluated on context-specific material; evaluated information covered topics such as informed consent (n = 18), health prevention (n = 8), and disease management (n = 2). Medical areas of interest were equally distributed among the following categories: “surgery” (n = 8) [28, 32, 36, 38, 41, 42, 44, 45], “cancer” (n = 8) [11, 25, 27, 29, 30, 34, 46] and “procedure” (n = 8) [22, 23, 26, 33, 37, 43, 47, 48]. The latter included colonoscopy, contrast administration, prostate vaporization, and urological and laser treatment, whereas cardiovascular diseases were investigated in four studies [24, 31, 35, 39].

Description of visual-based interventions

The preferred format for visual-based intervention was the video (n = 20), followed by multimedia-based presentations (n = 3), graphs (n = 1), and a booklet combined with either a video (n = 2), a multimedia-based presentation (n = 1), or a mind map (n = 1). The types of visual cues and their frequencies are summarized in the Appendix (Table S3). The average length of the video, calculated on 17 studies, was 10.88 ± 7.347 min (ranging from 2 to 30 min). Information regarding the language used in the videos was provided only in two studies, including English, Spanish, and Kiswahili [25]. If not reported, a local or national language was likely used.

The reading level of the video material was provided in three studies, and it was equal to 8th [26, 43] or 7th grade [41]. Concerning the multimedia-based presentation of material, it was impossible to determine the material’s average length: two studies reported data with different units of measurement, minutes [28] or just the number of slides [32] whereas the others did not disclose any information regarding the length of the material. Similarly, the reading level was provided only in one study, which was 7th grade [32].

Likewise, it was not possible to provide aggregated data on the length and reading level of the booklet format. The units of measurement, reported only in two studies, were not homogeneous: 18 min [46], 12 pages [11], and 2407 words [30]. Eventually, a mind map (n = 1) and a graph (n = 1) were the least represented formats. However, there was a lack of informative data concerning these strategies.

Description of outcome measures

Comprehension of health related material was the primary investigated outcome. However, this variable was measured differently across studies and included studies usually assessed patients’ level of comprehension through questionnaires explicitly developed for the study. Questionnaires were in the format of multiple choice (n = 10), true/false questions (n = 9), combined approach (multiple choice + true/false) (n = 2), and in some cases, computational questions, which required individuals to perform basic mathematical tasks regarding the health-related topic. However, it is worth noting that some studies (n = 6) did not disclose any information concerning the type of assessment. The individual’s level of comprehension was generally obtained by summing up the scores attributed to each correct answer. Occasionally, the final score was converted into a percentage. Furthermore, besides comprehension, three studies [11, 27, 47] measured participants’ level of HL with validated instruments: Test of Functional Health Literacy Ability – shortened (S-TOFHLA), Brief Health Literacy Scale (BHLS), Newest Vital Sign (NVS) and Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM) (Table S4).

Results of the systematic review

Overall, visual-based interventions were generally described as an effective instrument for enhancing the comprehension of health-related material compared to traditional methods (e.g., written material). In particular, in fourteen studies out of 22 (20 video-only + 2 videos combined with booklet) that adopted video material, participants in the intervention group (video format) had significantly higher comprehension levels than those in the control group (traditional method). Among the remaining six studies, two reported improvements in the level of comprehension after exposure to both video and standard material. However, the difference between the study arms was still not significant.

Among the studies that adopted multimedia-based presentation (n = 3), two registered a significant difference between study arms and showed a higher comprehension level than the control group. Instead, no significant difference was found in a study evaluating the effectiveness of charts (bar graph). Eventually, among the studies evaluating the effectiveness of booklets (n = 4), only one reported that participants provided with such format had higher comprehension levels than those provided with either leaflets or videos.

Meta-analytic results

Different meta-analyses were performed to compare video effectiveness to traditional information delivery methods. Due to the paucity of available data, studies including formats like multimedia-based presentations, charts, booklets, and mind maps were excluded. Also, among the studies adopting videos as a visual intervention tool (n = 20), some were removed due to the lack of data regarding mean and standard deviation (n = 9) or the study design (n = 1). As a result, ten studies were included in the meta-analyses for a total of 1784 participants. Three meta-analyses were performed in order to compare videos with (1) traditional methods, (2) written communication, and (3) verbal discussion with the doctor.

The comprehension level was significantly higher among participants who received a video than those provided with traditional methods. In particular, a medium effect size was found when comparing video materials with the standard method of information delivery (without discrimination of the type) (k = 10, Hedge’s g = 0.55, 95% CI [0.35, 0.75], p < 0.001, see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Meta-analysis: video vs traditional methods

Similarly, a medium effect size was also found in the case of videos compared to written information (k = 5, Hedge’s g = 0.65, 95% CI, [0.48, 0.82], p < 0.001, see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Meta-analysis: video vs. written information

Conversely, no significant difference was found when videos were compared only to oral discussion with the doctor (k = 6, Hedge’s g = 0.36, 95% CI, [-0.06, 0.77], p = 0.09, see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Meta-analysis: video vs. oral discussion

The level of heterogeneity was considerably high across the three meta-analyses. The former and the last meta-analysis reported high levels of inconsistency (I2 = 69%, p = 0.0006 and I2 = 82%, p < 0.0001). Instead, the inconsistency index was not significant in the second meta-analysis (I2 = 33%, p = 0.20). These results could be due to substantial differences in sample sizes, study design, the material used for the intervention, topic of interest, medical area investigated, and the use of different measures to assess patients’ levels of comprehension.

Risk of bias

In general studies showed a low risk of bias, except for the selection bias, for which a small proportion of studies (n = 4) is stated to be unclear. This result is mainly due to the study design: two studies [29, 34] adopted a quasi-experimental approach and one a pre-post study design [25]. The remaining study was evaluated as medium risk of bias because of significant baseline differences between the study arms, which may indicate failure in the randomisation process. The good quality of the studies is mainly related to homogenous setting, the possibility to immediately assess the outcome soon after the intervention and the objectivity in the measurement, which avoided outcome assessors to provide a personal and subjective evaluation.

Discussion

The current systematic review aims to evaluate the effectiveness of visual-based interventions to improve patients’ HL and levels of comprehension of health related material. To our knowledge, this is the first attempt to systematically and meta-analytically summarize evidence on the performance of these tools in the context of HL and knowledge of health information. According to qualitative results, visual-based interventions generally represented an effective way to improve comprehension of health-related information. However, their effectiveness mainly depends on the format employed. In particular, out of the 28 included studies, the most favorable evidence is provided by the studies that used videos as intervention tools (n = 20). More than half reported a significant increase in participants’ levels of comprehension.

Compared with traditional methods, participants provided with video material showed higher comprehension levels, with medium effect size, than those with standard oral or written material. In line with the Dual Coding Theory [14], the effectiveness of visual-based interventions is mainly due to their increased interactivity [49] and reduced reading efforts [50]. Indeed, conversely to traditional communication strategies, visual tools involve greater patient participation [51]. For instance, more interactivity is conveyed by quizzes and short games embedded in some visual tools, thus augmenting comprehension [52]. This is in line with previous research on the effect of interactivity on comprehension and attitude in the healthcare context [53], according to which the level of interactivity predicted patient comprehension. Even the absence of words, either partial or total, may have contributed to significant improvements in the outcome. Functional HL, a basic component of HL which includes basic skills of reading and numeracy [54], dramatically impacts an individual’s ability to understand and appraise health-related material. Indeed, visual cues require less effort to be processed and recalled than written information; this may make information more accessible and comprehensible to those with limited HL [50]. Eventually, the effectiveness of these instruments could be explained by referring to the Dual Coding Theory [14]. Our results further show that the “picture superiority effect” owned by visual cues allows distinguishing these tools from more traditional methods (i.e., written communication) since the multimedia content embedded into these tools (i.e., video, images, audio) was subjected to a double encoding process [14], which in turn allowed the stimulus to be consolidated, thus improving the comprehension and recall of the information.

However, the difference was no longer significant when the video format was confronted with an oral discussion with a physician. The lack of a significant difference between video and verbal discussion could be attributed to the benefits of face-to-face consultations, such as the possibility to interpret non-verbal messages [55] and directly assess patient’s comprehension level [56]. Non-verbal communication is essential in the doctor-patient interaction [57]. Thanks to the possibility of perceiving patients’ emotional states through the analysis of gestures, postures, and mimics, the physician can adjust the conversation to make the patient feel more confident with the information received in terms of content and form. In practice, the doctor may avoid complex medical terminology to reduce cognitive efforts on behalf of the patient and simplify the comprehension process [56, 58]. Similar strategies, like the “teach-back method” [56], involve the immediate assessment of the patient’s level of comprehension. Adopting such solutions might have noticeably reduced the gap in comprehension between the intervention (video) and control group (oral discussion), which eventually resulted in a non-significant difference. Hence, oral discussion with a doctor may represent a valuable and complementary intervention to be implemented for patients with low health literacy.

Finally, some remaining studies reported increases between pre-and post-intervention, though not significant. In particular, concerning multimedia-based presentations, two studies out of three registered a significant difference between the intervention and control groups. However, due to the low number of studies included in this category, it is not accurate to conclude that multimedia-based presentations are valuable tools for improving comprehension of medical material. Conversely, charts were ineffective. However, their use was investigated by only one study. Hence, future studies are needed to better explore the effectiveness of multimedia material in addition to the video.

Based on these findings, we encourage the adoption of visual tools for medical information purposes to set up innovative, effective, and fair communication. Nevertheless, we call for more research reporting more details on these interventions' characteristics, graphical layouts, and designs. Also, more research is needed regarding different medical areas and contexts, chronic and acute illnesses, and mental health problems.

Limitations

The present study has some limitations. First, the lack of validated scales for measuring comprehension of medical material might have introduced biases in the analyses and augmented the heterogeneity of the meta-analytic results. Indeed, each of the included studies measured the outcome through different scales. The heterogeneity found could also be attributed to variations in sample size and participants’ characteristics, including age, gender, ethnicity, and educational level. Second, included studies were not equally distributed across the different types of visual interventions. As a result, we could not draw robust conclusions on the effectiveness of some tools (i.e., charts, booklets, and mind maps). Third, included studies were mainly from limited geographical regions. Only a few were conducted in low- and middle-income countries, where adopting such instruments may represent an effective tool to improve people’s well-being and state of health [59]. Moreover, the absence of studies from South America may represent a methodological and research bias, that may have significantly limited the landscape of the included studies. Additionally, our study might be limited due to the lack of consideration of specific terms related to interactive visual resources (e.g., video). Hence, we suggest that future studies should include detailed descriptions of visual- and video- based interventions.

Finally, we call for future research to develop specific comprehension scales to increase and facilitate comparability across studies. Moreover, more data are needed to compare the mere use of videos with the combination of video and oral discussion, since adopting this mixed approach is a promising strategy for improving, even more, the comprehension of medical material. Finally, it would be useful to investigate the effectiveness of visual-based intervention in different clinical settings and populations, including young people. Antelo and colleagues [60] investigated the health literacy of women in Argentina regarding Human Papilloma Virus (HPV) after using a mobile counseling app and found that a mobile app is a good tool to help HPV-positive women by providing information and reducing fears [60]. Lee and colleagues [61] analyzed the effect of user-centered mHealth intervention apps (e.g., Click to Connect, PLANET MassCONECT, and SmartPhone App for Public Health) to inform future work regarding app designing in Massachusetts and found that one of the critical features that increase HL among underserved communities is to design apps with usability, readability, and navigability in mind. Indeed, the vast majority of the studies in the review regarded the informed consent process, whereas publications on disease management and health prevention are still scarce. Regarding the need to investigate the effectiveness of similar instrument in other domains and population, Ito-Jaeger and collegues, in their scoping review on 17 studies, illuminated the effectiveness of digital video interventions as powerful tool in enhancing mental health literacy among young individuals [18].

Conclusion

Visual-based interventions are effective tools for improving patients’ levels of comprehension. In particular, the adoption of video formats significantly augments comprehension compared to more traditional methods. However, no additional benefits were found compared to the oral discussion with a physician. Future studies should investigate whether the combination of these methods (visual-based intervention + oral discussions with a physician) is more effective than the simple provision of the video format.

Availability of data and materials

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Abbreviations

HL:

Health Literacy

S-TOFHLA:

Test of Functional Health Literacy Ability Shortened

BHLS:

Brief Health Literacy Scale

NVS:

Newest Vital Sign

REALM:

Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine

References

*=articles included in the systematic review

  1. Ratzan S, Parker R, Selden C, Zorn M. National library of medicine current bibliographies in medicine: health literacy. Bethesda: National Institutes of Health; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Levy H, Janke A. Health literacy and access to care. J Health Commun. 2016;21(Suppl 1):43–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2015.1131776.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Baccolini V, Rosso A, Di Paolo C, Isonne C, Salerno C, Migliara G, Prencipe GP, Massimi A, Marzuillo C, De Vito C, Villari P, Romano F. What is the prevalence of low health literacy in European Union Member States? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Gen Intern Med. 2021;36(3):753–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-020-06407-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Kutner M, Greenburg E, Jin Y, Paulsen C. The Health Literacy of America’s Adults: Results from the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy. NCES 2006–483. In National Center for Education Statistics. ED Pubs, P. 2006. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED493284.

  5. Lopez C, Kim B, Sacks K. Health literacy in the United States: enhancing assessments and reducing disparities. SSRN. 2022. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4182046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Rajah R, Hassali MAA, Murugiah MK. A systematic review of the prevalence of limited health literacy in Southeast Asian countries. Public Health. 2019;167:8–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2018.09.028.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Chew LD, Bradley KA, Flum DR, Cornia PB, Koepsell TD. The impact of low health literacy on surgical practice. Am J Surg. 2004;188(3):250–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amjsurg.2004.04.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cho YI, Lee SYD, Arozullah AM, Crittenden KS. Effects of health literacy on health status and health service utilization amongst the elderly. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(8):1809–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Berkman N, Sheridan SL, Donahue KE, Halpern DJ, Crotty K. Low health literacy and health outcomes: an updated systematic review. Ann Intern Med. 2011 https://www.acpjournals.org/doi/10.7326/0003-4819-155-2-201107190-00005.

  10. Eichler K, Wieser S, Brügger U. The costs of limited health literacy: A systematic review. Int J Public Health. 2009;54(5):313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-009-0058-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Housten AJ, Kamath GR, Bevers TB, Cantor SB, Dixon N, Hite A, Kallen MA, Leal VB, Li L, Volk RJ. Does animation improve comprehension of risk information in patients with low health literacy? A randomized trial. Med Decis Making. 2020;40(1):17–28. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X19890296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. CDC. Visual Communication resources for health-related materials. Centers for disease control and prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/healthliteracy/developmaterials/visual-communication.html. Accessed 29 April 2021.

  13. Crutcher RJ, Beer JM. An auditory analog of the picture superiority effect. Mem Cognit. 2011;39(1):63–74. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0015-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Paivio A. Imagery and verbal processes. Psychology Press. 2013. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315798868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Katz MG, Kripalani S, Weiss BD. Use of pictorial aids in medication instructions: A review of the literature. Am J Health Syst Pharm. 2006;63(23):2391–7. https://doi.org/10.2146/ajhp060162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Glaser J, Nouri S, Fernandez A, Sudore RL, Schillinger D, Klein-Fedyshin M, Schenker Y. Interventions to improve patient comprehension in informed consent for medical and surgical procedures: an updated systematic review. Med Decis Making. 2020;40(2):119–43. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X19896348.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Lee TW, Lee SH, Kim HH, Kang SJ. Effective intervention strategies to improve health outcomes for cardiovascular disease patients with low health literacy skills: a systematic review. Asian Nurs Res. 2012;6(4):128–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anr.2012.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Ito-Jaeger S, Perez Vallejos E, Curran T, Spors V, Long Y, Liguori A, ... & Crawford P. Digital video interventions and mental health literacy among young people: a scoping review. J Ment Health. 2022;31(6):873–883.

  19. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, Shamseer L, Tetzlaff JM, Akl EA, Brennan SE, Chou R, Glanville J, Grimshaw JM, Hróbjartsson A, Lalu MM, Li T, Loder EW, Mayo-Wilson E, McDonald S, … Moher D. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021; 372: n71. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71

  20. Higgins JP, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Sterne JA. Assessing risk of bias in a randomized trial. In Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 2019; 205–228. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.ch8.

  21. Deeks JJ, Higgins JP, Altman DG, & Group, on behalf of the C. S. M. Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses. In Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 2019;241–284. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.ch10.

  22. * Agre P, Kurtz RC, Krauss BJ. A randomized trial using videotape to present consent information for colonoscopy. Gastrointest Endosc. 1994;40(3):271–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0016-5107(94)70054-0.

  23. * Baenninger PB, Faes L, Kaufmann C, Reichmuth V, Bachmann LM, Thiel MA. Efficiency of video-presented information about excimer laser treatment on ametropic patients’ knowledge and satisfaction with the informed consent process. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2018;44(12):1426–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2018.07.052.

  24. Bowers N, Eisenberg E, Montbriand J, Jaskolka J, Roche-Nagle G. Using a multimedia presentation to improve patient understanding and satisfaction with informed consent for minimally invasive vascular procedures. Surgeon. 2017;15(1):7–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.surge.2015.09.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cooper EC, Maher JA, Naaseh A, Crawford EW, Chinn JO, Runge AS, Lucas AN, Zezoff DC, Bera KR, Dinicu AI, White KM, Tewari SE, Hari A, Bernstein M, Chang J, Ziogas A, Pearre DC, Tewari KS. Implementation of human papillomavirus video education for women participating in mass cervical cancer screening in Tanzania. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2021;224(1):105.e1–105.e9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2020.07.018.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Cowan EA, Calderon Y, Gennis P, Macklin R, Ortiz C, Wall SP. Spanish and English video-assisted informed consent for intravenous contrast administration in the emergency department: A randomized controlled trial. Ann Emerg Med. 2007; 49(2):221–230, 230.e1–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annemergmed.2006.07.934.

  27. Donelle L, Hoffman-Goetz L, Gatobu S, Arocha JF. Comprehension of Internet-based numeric cancer information by older adults. Inform Health Soc Care. 2009;34(4):209–24. https://doi.org/10.3109/17538150903358552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Ellett L, Villegas R, Beischer A, Ong N, Maher P. Use of a multimedia module to aid the informed consent process in patients undergoing gynecologic laparoscopy for pelvic pain: Randomized controlled trial. J Minim Invasive Gynecol. 2014;21(4):602–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmig.2014.01.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Frosch DL, Légaré F, Mangione CM. Using decision aids in community-based primary care: A theory-driven evaluation with ethnically diverse patients. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73(3):490–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2008.07.040.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Gattellari M, Ward JE. A community-based randomised controlled trial of three different educational resources for men about prostate cancer screening. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;57(2):168–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2004.05.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. * Goldberger JJ, Kruse J, Kadish AH, Passman R, Bergner DW. Effect of informed consent format on patient anxiety, knowledge, and satisfaction. Am Heart J. 2011;162(4):780-785.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ahj.2011.07.006.

  32. Gyomber D, Lawrentschuk N, Wong P, Parker F, Bolton DM. Improving informed consent for patients undergoing radical prostatectomy using multimedia techniques: A prospective randomized crossover study. BJU Int. 2010;106(8):1152–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-410X.2010.09309.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Ham DY, Choi WS, Song SH, Ahn YJ, Park HK, Kim HG, Son H. Prospective randomized controlled study on the efficacy of multimedia informed consent for patients scheduled to undergo green-light high-performance system photoselective vaporization of the prostate. World J Mens Health. 2016;34(1):47–55. https://doi.org/10.5534/wjmh.2016.34.1.47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. * Ilic D, Egberts K, McKenzie JE, Risbridger G, Green S. Informing men about prostate cancer screening: A randomized controlled trial of patient education materials. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(4):466–71. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-007-0466-z.

  35. * Lattuca B, Barber-Chamoux N, Alos B, Sfaxi A, Mulliez A, Miton N, Levasseur T, Servoz C, Derimay F, Hachet O, Motreff P, Metz D, Lairez O, Mewton N, Belle L, Akodad M, Mathivet T, Ecarnot F, Pollet J, … INFOCORO investigators. Impact of video on the understanding and satisfaction of patients receiving informed consent before elective inpatient coronary angiography: A randomized trial. Am Heart J. 2018; 200:67–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ahj.2018.03.006.

  36. * Lin YK, Chen CW, Lee WC, Cheng YC, Lin TY, Lin CJ, Shi L, Tien YC, Kuo LC. Educational video-assisted versus conventional informed consent for trauma-related debridement surgery: A parallel group randomized controlled trial. BMC Med Ethics. 2018;19(1):23. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12910-018-0264-7.

  37. Luck A, Pearson S, Maddern G, Hewett P. Effects of video information on precolonoscopy anxiety and knowledge: A randomised trial. Lancet (London, England). 1999;354(9195):2032–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(98)10495-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Mason V, McEwan A, Walker D, Barrett S, James D. The use of video information in obtaining consent for female sterilisation: A randomised study. BJOG. 2003;110(12):1062–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. * Mazor KM, Baril J, Dugan E, Spencer F, Burgwinkle P, Gurwitz JH. Patient education about anticoagulant medication: Is narrative evidence or statistical evidence more effective? Patient Educ Couns. 2007;69(1–3):145–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2007.08.010.

  40. Nagamma T, Ashok L, Konuri A, Chandrasekaran V. Effectiveness of audio-visual and print media intervention on knowledge of cervical health among rural women in Southern India. Niger Postgrad Med J. 2020;27(4):343–7. https://doi.org/10.4103/npmj.npmj_148_20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Rossi M, McClellan R, Chou L, Davis K. Informed consent for ankle fracture surgery: Patient comprehension of verbal and videotaped information. Foot Ankle Int. 2004;25(10):756–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/107110070402501011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. * Rossi MJ, Guttmann D, MacLennan MJ, Lubowitz JH. Video informed consent improves knee arthroscopy patient comprehension. Arthroscopy. 2005;21(6):739–743 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arthro.2005.02.015.

  43. Shaw MJ, Beebe TJ, Tomshine PA, Adlis SA, Cass OW. A randomized, controlled trial of interactive, multimedia software for patient colonoscopy education. J Clin Gastroenterol. 2001;32(2):142–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004836-200102000-00010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. * Shukla AN, Daly MK, Legutko P. Informed consent for cataract surgery: Patient understanding of verbal, written, and videotaped information. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2012;38(1):80–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2011.07.030.

  45. * Vo TA, Ngai P, Tao JP. A randomized trial of multimedia-facilitated informed consent for cataract surgery. Clinical Ophthalmology (Auckland, N.Z.). 2018; 12, 1427–1432. https://doi.org/10.2147/OPTH.S150670.

  46. Volk RJ, Jibaja-Weiss ML, Hawley ST, Kneuper S, Spann SJ, Miles BJ, Hyman DJ. Entertainment education for prostate cancer screening: A randomized trial among primary care patients with low health literacy. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73(3):482–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2008.07.033.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Walker D, Adebajo A, Heslop P, Hill J, Firth J, Bishop P, Helliwell PS. Patient education in rheumatoid arthritis: The effectiveness of the ARC booklet and the mind map. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2007;46(10):1593–6. https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/kem171.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Winter M, Kam J, Nalavenkata S, Hardy E, Handmer M, Ainsworth H, Lee WG, Louie-Johnsun M. The use of portable video media vs standard verbal communication in the urological consent process: A multicentre, randomised controlled, crossover trial. BJU Int. 2016;118(5):823–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/bju.13595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Patwardhan M, Murthy S. When does higher degree of interaction lead to higher learning in visualizations? Exploring the role of ‘Interactivity Enriching Features.’ Comput Educ. 2015;82:292–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2014.11.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Dewan P. Words versus pictures: leveraging the research on visual communication. Partnership Can J Lib Info Pract Res. 2015;10(1). https://doi.org/10.21083/partnership.v10i1.3137

  51. Lajoie SP, Azevedo R. Teaching and learning in technology-rich environments. In: Handbook of educational psychology. Mahwah (New Jersey): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2006. p. 803–21.

  52. Chou C. Interactivity and interactive functions in web-based learning systems: A technical framework for designers. Br J Edu Technol. 2003;34(3):265–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8535.00326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Lustria MLA. Can interactivity make a difference? Effects of interactivity on the comprehension of and attitudes toward online health content. J Am Soc Inform Sci Technol. 2007;58(6):766–76. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.20557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Schulz PJ, Nakamoto K. Health literacy and patient empowerment in health communication: The importance of separating conjoined twins. Patient Educ Couns. 2013;90(1):4–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2012.09.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Roter DL, Frankel RM, Hall JA, Sluyter D. The expression of emotion through nonverbal behavior in medical visits. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21(1):28–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00306.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Oates DJ, Paasche-Orlow MK. Health literacy: Communication strategies to improve patient comprehension of cardiovascular health. Circulation. 2009;119(7):1049–51. https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.818468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Silverman J, Kinnersley P. Doctors’non-verbal behaviour in consultations: Look at the patient before you look at the computer. Br J Gen Pract. 2010;60(571):76–8. https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgp10X482293.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Houts PS, Doak CC, Doak LG, Loscalzo MJ. The role of pictures in improving health communication: A review of research on attention, comprehension, recall, and adherence. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;61(2):173–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. McCool J, Dobson R, Whittaker R, Paton C. Mobile health (mHealth) in low- and middle-income countries. Annu Rev Public Health. 2022;43(1):525–39. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-052620-093850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Sanchez Antelo V, Szwarc L, Paolino M, Saimovici D, Massaccesi S, Viswanath K, Arrossi S. A Counseling mobile app to reduce the psychosocial impact of human papillomavirus testing: formative research using a user-centered design approach in a low-middle-income setting in Argentina. JMIR Form Res. 2022;6(1):e32610. https://doi.org/10.2196/32610.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Lee EW, McCloud RF, Viswanath K. Designing effective eHealth interventions for underserved groups: five lessons from a decade of eHealth intervention design and deployment. J Med Internet Res. 2022;24(1):e25419. https://doi.org/10.2196/25419.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was partially supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation under Grant P500PS_202974.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EG, LM, and PJS contributed to the conception of the work; EG contributed to the acquisition and analysis of data. EG, LM, and PJS contributed to the interpretation of data. EG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. LM and PJS substantively revised it. All authors have approved the submitted version.

Authors’ information

Peter J. Schulz is Director of the Institute of Communication and Health at the University of Lugano, Switzerland and Professor of Communication Theories and Health Communication.

Laura Marciano is a post-doctoral research fellow at the Harvard Chan T.H. School of Public Health, Boston (USA). Her research is focused on digital media use and well-being.

Elisa Galmarini is, at the time of the draft of the manuscript, a student in the master course “Communication, Management and Health” at the University of Lugano, Switzerland.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Johannes Schulz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Galmarini, E., Marciano, L. & Schulz, P.J. The effectiveness of visual-based interventions on health literacy in health care: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Health Serv Res 24, 718 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-024-11138-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-024-11138-1

Keywords